5-Hydroxytryptamine 1A Receptor Blockade Facilitates Aversive ...

3 downloads 145562 Views 481KB Size Report
Aversive Learning in Mice: Interactions with Cholinergic and. Glutamatergic ..... Fremont, CA) at 2000 dots per inch and processed using the Adobe. Photoshop ...
0022-3565/06/3162-581–591$20.00 THE JOURNAL OF PHARMACOLOGY AND EXPERIMENTAL THERAPEUTICS Copyright © 2006 by The American Society for Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics JPET 316:581–591, 2006

Vol. 316, No. 2 92262/3071926 Printed in U.S.A.

5-Hydroxytryptamine 1A Receptor Blockade Facilitates Aversive Learning in Mice: Interactions with Cholinergic and Glutamatergic Mechanisms Nather Madjid, Elin Elvander Tottie, Maria Lu¨ttgen, Bjo¨rn Meister, Johan Sandin, Alexander Kuzmin, Oliver Stiedl, and Sven Ove O¨gren Department of Neuroscience, Division of Behavioral Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden (N.M., E.E.T., M.L., B.M., J.S., A.K., S.O.O¨.); Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Karolinska University Hospital, Stockholm, Sweden (M.L.); and Department of Functional Genomics, Center for Neurogenomics and Cognitive Research, Free University, Amsterdam, The Netherlands (O.S.) Received July 8, 2005; accepted October 12, 2005

ABSTRACT The effects of 5-hydroxytryptamine 1A (5-HT1A) receptor ligands on aversive learning were examined in the passive avoidance (PA) task in mice. Anxiety and autonomic functions were investigated using the elevated plus-maze and heart rate measurements. The main findings from this study are as follows. 1) Pretraining administration of the 5-HT1A receptor agonist 8-OH-DPAT [8-hydroxy-2-(di-n-propylamino)tetralin hydrobromide] facilitated PA retention at low doses (0.01 and 0.03 mg/kg) but impaired PA retention at higher doses (0.1–1.0 mg/kg), consistent with previous findings in the rat. 2) Similar to the acetylcholinesterase inhibitor physostigmine, pretraining administration of the 5-HT1A receptor antagonists [(R)-3-N,Ndicyclobutylamino-8 fluoro-3,4-dihydro-3H-1-benzopyran-5carboxamide hydrogen(2R,3R)-tartrate monohydrate] NAD-299 (0.1–2 mg/kg) and [N-2-4-(2-methoxyphenyl)-1-piperazinylethyl-N-(2-pyridinyl)cyclohexane carboxamide trihydrochloride] WAY-100635 (0.3–3 mg/kg) enhanced PA retention. 3) The impairment (1 mg/kg) but not the facilitation (0.03 mg/kg) induced by 8-OH-DPAT was fully blocked by NAD-299 (0.3 mg/

kg). 4) 5-HT1A receptor ligands given immediate post-training failed to alter PA retention. 5) NAD-299 (0.3–1 mg/kg) blocked the impairment of PA retention caused by a) the nonselective muscarinic receptor antagonist scopolamine and b) the noncompetitive N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor antagonist MK-801 [(5R,10S)-(⫹)-5-methyl-10,11-dihydro-5H-dibenzo(a,d)cyclohepten-5,10-imine hydrogen maleate]. 6) A subthreshold dose of scopolamine completely blocked the facilitatory effect of NAD-299 on PA retention. 7) Anxiety-related behaviors and autonomic function were unchanged by NAD-299. 8) In situ hybridization showed that septal neurons expressing 5-HT1A receptor mRNA were codistributed with markers for cholinergic, GABAergic, and glutamatergic neurons. These results indicate that systemic administration of 5-HT1A receptor antagonists can facilitate cognitive performance, most likely by enhancing hippocampal/cortical cholinergic and glutamatergic neurotransmissions. Selective 5-HT1A receptor antagonists may be useful in the treatment of cognitive deficits such as Alzheimer’s disease.

This work was supported by grants from the Swedish Medical Research Council (Vetenskapsrådet; Project Numbers 14X-11588 and 72X-10358), Karolinska Institutets fonder, Wallenberg Consortium North, Alzheimerfonden, Marianne and Marcus Wallenberg’s Foundation, and Stiftelsen Gamla Tja¨narinnor. This work has previously been presented in a thesis by M. Lu¨ttgen from the Karolinska Institutet, Department of Neuroscience: Lu¨ttgen M (2004) Serotonergic receptor subtypes in learning and memory, in Focus on 5-HT1A, 5-HT1B and 5-HT2A Receptors. Larserics Digital Print AB, Stockholm, Sweden. Article, publication date, and citation information can be found at http://jpet.aspetjournals.org. doi:10.1124/jpet.105.092262.

Serotonin (5-hydroxytryptamine; 5-HT) is a biogenic amine involved in a wide range of physiological functions, including ¨ gren, 1985; Buhot et al., 2000). The learning and memory (O ascending 5-HT neurons originating from the brainstem raphe nuclei innervate virtually all regions of the rat forebrain, allowing for widespread neuromodulatory actions (Dahlstro¨m and Fuxe, 1964) mediated by 14 identified receptor subtypes (Hoyer et al., 2002). Among the 5-HT receptor subtypes implicated in cognitive functions, the 5-HT1A receptors are of special interest because they are abundant in brain

ABBREVIATIONS: 5-HT, 5-hydroxytryptamine (serotonin); PA, passive avoidance; 8-OH-DPAT, 8-hydroxy-2-(di-n-propylamino)tetralin hydrobromide; NAD-299, (R)-3-N,N-dicyclobutylamino-8 fluoro-3,4-dihydro-3H-1-benzopyran-5-carboxamide hydrogen(2R,3R)-tartrate monohydrate; WAY-100635, N-2-4-(2-methoxyphenyl)-1-piperazinylethyl-N-(2-pyridinyl)cyclohexane carboxamide trihydrochloride; MK-801, (5R,10S)-(⫹)-5methyl-10,11-dihydro-5H-dibenzo(a,d)cyclohepten-5,10-imine hydrogen maleate; ECG, electrocardiogram; ACh, cholinergic; VAChT, vesicular acetylcholine transporter; US, unconditioned stimulus; MSDB, medial septum/diagonal band of Broca; VGLUT2, vesicular glutamate transporter 2; ANOVA, analysis of variance; NAN-190, 1-(2-methoxyphenyl)-4-[4-(2-phthalimido)butyl]piperazine. 581

582

Madjid et al.

areas associated with cognitive functions, such as the cerebral cortex, hippocampus, and septum (Chalmers and Watson, 1991; Pompeiano et al., 1992). Moreover, the neuronal localization of 5-HT1A receptors allows for modulation of both 5-HT neuronal activity as well as several other neurotransmitter systems. Stimulation of presynaptic 5-HT1A receptors (autoreceptors) localized on cell bodies in the midbrain raphe nuclei reduces raphe neuronal firing and 5-HT release (Sprouse and Aghajanian, 1987; Chalmers and Watson, 1991), resulting in reduced 5-HT neurotransmission in 5-HT terminal areas. In contrast, activation of 5-HT1A receptors localized on cell bodies and dendrites of neurons postsynaptic to 5-HT nerve terminals hyperpolarizes the innervated cells (for review, see Aghajanian, 1995). The functional significance of the 5-HT1A receptor in cognition is partly based on the use of 5-HT1A receptor agonists. Pretraining administration of partial agonists at the 5-HT1A receptors, such as buspirone and tandospirone, disrupted acquisition of behavior in various models (Winsauer et al., 1999). Because the effects of partial 5-HT1A agonists on learning seem to depend on their relative efficacy at presynaptic versus postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptors (Winsauer et al., 1999), the use of full 5-HT1A receptor agonist, such as 8-OHDPAT (Foreman et al., 1993), is critical for exploring the physiological role of 5-HT1A receptors. Pretraining administration of 8-OH-DPAT (Sanger and Joly, 1989; Rowan et al., ¨ gren, 2000; Lu¨ttgen et 1990; Carli et al., 1993; Misane and O al., 2005a) impaired learning and memory performance in a dose-dependent manner in the passive avoidance (PA) task in the rat. 8-OH-DPAT also retarded spatial learning in the water maze (Carli et al., 1992; Kant et al., 1998) and radial maze tasks (Winter and Petti, 1987). The impairments caused by 8-OH-DPAT in the rat seemed to be related to a stimulation of postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptors (Carli et al., ¨ gren, 2003). Thus, 1995; Misane et al., 1998; Misane and O 5-HT1A receptor antagonists fully blocked the 5-HT1A receptor agonist-mediated impairments, thereby supporting the involvement of the 5-HT1A receptor in the action of the agonists (Carli et al., 1995; Misane et al., 1998). In contrast, facilitated learning and memory have been observed after low, presumably presynaptic, doses of 8-OH-DPAT both in the PA task (Lu¨ttgen et al., 2005a) and in an operant-delayed matching to position task in the rat (Cole et al., 1994). Pretraining administration of 5-HT1A receptor antagonists in rats has reported either facilitation (Sanger and Joly, 1989; Belcheva et al., 1997; Pitsikas et al., 2003), impairment (Cole et al., 1994), or no effects (Carli et al., 1997; Misane and ¨ gren, 2003) on learning performance in various tasks. O There are limited studies on the role of the 5-HT1A receptor in cognition in mice. Similar to findings in the rat, 8-OHDPAT, when injected before but not after training, caused an impairment in PA retention (Mendelson et al., 1993). Studies using pretraining administration of 5-HT1A receptor antagonists in mice have reported variable results, including no effects ((Mendelson et al., 1993; Stiedl et al., 2000b) or impairments (Galeotti et al., 2000) in the PA and fear-conditioning task. In addition, results obtained after post-training administration of 5-HT1A receptor antagonists are inconsistent because both facilitation (Schneider et al., 2003) and no effects (Manuel-Apolinar and Meneses, 2004) have been reported. These inconsistent findings may be explained by differential effects on presynaptic and postsynaptic 5-HT1A re-

ceptors, differences in routes of administration, and lack of receptor specificity of the compounds used as well as different designs of the studies. In addition, difference in species may contribute to the lack of clear answers regarding the physiological role of the 5-HT1A receptor in cognition. The main aim of this study was to examine the role of basal 5-HT1A receptor-mediated transmission by the use of two selective 5-HT1A receptor antagonists, NAD-299 (Johansson et al., 1997) and WAY-100635 (Fletcher et al., 1996). The ability of the 5-HT1A receptor antagonists to influence PA performance was investigated alone or in combination with 8-OH-DPAT to explore the involvement of presynaptic versus postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptors. Immediate post-training administration of the 5-HT1A receptor ligands investigated the possible involvement of 5-HT1A receptors in memory consolidation. Because the 5-HT1A receptor has been implicated in emotional functions, the ability of 5-HT1A receptor blockade to affect anxiety-related behavior and autonomic responses was also studied. In situ hybridization of sections of the mouse septum was used to provide an anatomical basis for interactions between the 5-HT1A receptors and cholinergic/glutamatergic neurons in the septohippocampal projection.

Materials and Methods Materials Animals. Adult male NMRI mice (2 months of age, weight 20 –30 g; Scanbur, Sollentuna, Sweden) were used in most experiments. Adult male C57BL/6J mice (2 months of age, weight 25–30 g; Charles River, Sulzfeld, Germany) were used in the studies, which examined the potential effects of 5-HT1A receptor blockade on autonomic function (heart rate) and anxiety-like behavior. The interstrain comparison was included for the reason that the C57BL/6J strain is more sensitive to anxiety-provoking events than the NMRI strain. The use of such a strain will thus increase the sensitivity of the heart rate measurements (Stiedl et al., 1999; Griebel et al., 2000). Therefore, it was also logical to compare the effects of 5-HT1A antagonism on PA in the C57BL/6J versus the NMRI strain. The animals were housed in groups of four to six in standard Macrolon cages (type 3: 42 ⫻ 26 ⫻ 20 cm) in a temperature- and humidity-controlled room with a constant 12-h light/dark cycle (lights on at 7:00 AM) and free access to standard lab chow and tap water up to the time of experiments. To avoid postsurgery complications, animals used for heart rate measurements were individually housed postsurgery in standard Macrolon cages (type 2: 22 ⫻ 16 ⫻ 13 cm). The mice were habituated to the animal facilities and handled daily by the same experimenter for a period of five days before the experiments. Experiments were conducted in experimentally naive mice that were used only once to prevent possible carryover effects between tests. Experimental procedures involving animals and their care followed the provisions and recommendations of the Swedish animal protection legislation. The experimental procedures were approved by the local Animal Ethical Committees (Ethical Numbers N155/01, N48/02, N132/01, and 509.42502/02-01.00) and conformed to the European Council Directive (86/609/EEC). Drugs. The following drugs were used: ⫹/⫺8-OH-DPAT (AstraZeneca R&D, So¨derta¨lje, Sweden); robalzotan (NAD-299, AstraZeneca R&D); WAY-100635 (Wyeth, Taplow, UK); (⫺)-scopolamine hydrobromide (Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO); physostigmine hemisulfate (Eserine; Fluka, Buchs, Switzerland); MK-801 (SigmaAldrich). All drugs were dissolved in sterile saline (0.9% NaCl) and injected s.c. into the scruff of the neck at a volume of 4 ml/kg. Injections occurred at specific time points prior to training (40, 30,

5-HT1A Receptor Blockade Facilitates Aversive Learning 20, 15, 10, and 5 min) or immediately post-training as noted under Results. Saline control groups were run concurrently.

Methods Each experiment was carried out by a single operator. The experimenter was not blind to the pharmacological treatments given. Behavioral tests were conducted during the light phase between 10:00 AM and 3:00 PM. For habituation, mice were transferred to the experimental room 30 to 60 min prior to the experiment. Passive Avoidance. Passive avoidance is an associative learning paradigm, based on Pavlovian fear-conditioning and instrumental conditioning, and it is sensitive to changes in serotonergic, glutama¨ gren, 1985; Mistergic, and cholinergic (ACh) neurotransmissions (O ¨ gren, 2000). The PA task was conducted as described ane and O ¨ gren, 1986; Misane and O ¨ gren, 2000), with some earlier for rats (O important modifications adapted for mice. In brief, a two-compartment standard shuttle box (adapted for mice; 10 ⫻ 16 ⫻ 18 cm; Ugo Basile, Comerio-Varese, Italy), with two communicating compartments of equal size and a stainless steel bar floor, was used. The compartments were separated by a 4 ⫻ 4-cm sliding door built into the separating wall. One compartment (the conditioning compartment) was painted black to obtain a dark chamber, whereas the other compartment (light compartment) was illuminated by a light bulb (24 V, 5 watts). The light intensity in the light versus the dark compartment was 330 and 3 lux, respectively. The animals were handled daily for a period of five days prior to the experiments. Handling has been shown to reduce variations between animals in the PA task probably by a reduction in stress ¨ gren et al., unpublished results). Fifteen minutes before the first (O drug injection on day 1, mice were first allowed to explore the light compartment for 60 s and, thereafter, the sliding door was opened. Once the animal crossed to the dark compartment, the sliding door was automatically closed and the animals were allowed to explore the dark compartment for an additional 60 s. After the exploration period, the animals were removed from the PA apparatus and returned to a holding cage. To be able to study both enhancement and impairment of PA memory in the same experiment, two different shock intensities were used. To enable demonstration of facilitatory effects of drugs on PA retention, a mildly aversive electrical current (0.3 mA) served as the unconditioned stimulus (US) in the studies with NMRI mice. By this procedure, a relatively weak memory retention was predicted in the control group, allowing for detection of both facilitation and impairment of PA retention in the same experiment. In some experiments with NMRI mice, a current of 0.4 mA was used to explicitly study impairment of PA retention. This procedure provides a sufficiently dynamical range for impairment and blockade of impairment when combining agonist and antagonist treatment. PA training was conducted in a single session on day 1. The animals were treated with the test compounds as described below. After a defined time interval following injection, the animal was placed in the light compartment with the sliding door closed (i.e., no access to the dark compartment) and allowed to explore for 60 s. After 60 s, the sliding door was automatically opened and the mouse could cross over into the dark compartment. The latency to cross into the dark compartment (training latency) was recorded. Upon entering the dark compartment with all four paws, the sliding door was automatically closed and a weak electrical current was delivered via the grid floor (scrambled current: 2-s duration, 0.2-mA C57BL/6J and 0.3-mA NMRI). Some experiments used a current of 0.4 mA to explicitly study impairment of PA retention (see above). The reactions (e.g., vocalization, jumping) of the mice to the electric current were noted. After exposure to the US, the mouse remained for 30 s in the dark compartment of the PA apparatus before being removed and transferred to its holding cage. The rationale for this procedure is to avoid mice from an aversive association of the US with the handling (removal from the compartment after the US exposure).

583

After completion of testing, the mice were returned to their home cages. Retention latencies were determined 24 h after training (day 2). Each animal was placed into the light (safe) compartment, with access to the dark compartment within 10 s. The latency to cross into the dark compartment with all four paws was automatically measured (step-through latency; retention latency; cut-off time was 300 s). Elevated Plus-Maze. The apparatus was elevated 1 m above the floor. It consisted of four arms (30 ⫻ 5 cm) extending from the central area (5 ⫻ 5 cm) (TSE, Bad Homburg, Germany) made of dark gray polyvinyl chloride. Two opposite arms were enclosed by 10-cm high walls made from the same material, whereas two arms (open arm) remained without walls. The animal was gently released in the center of the plus-maze facing an open arm. The behavior of each mouse was monitored for 5 min by a video-tracking system (TSE). The main parameters recorded were time spent in open arms, closed arms, and central region; the total distance traveled during the experiment (locomotion index); and the number of entries to closed and opened arms (exploration index). Based on these results, the “anxiety index” was calculated as the ratio between the time spent in the closed arms and the total time of experiment. All boxes were thoroughly cleaned before each mouse was tested using 70% ethanol. Heart Rate Measurement. Electrocardiogram (ECG) transmitters (TA10EA-F20; Data Sciences, St. Paul, MN) were implanted i.p. with two electrodes placed s.c. as described previously (Stiedl and Spiess, 1997). The mice were allowed to recover from surgery for 14 to 21 days. Telemetry measurements were performed in the home cage 15 min after s.c. injection of NAD-299 (0.3 mg/kg). The s.c. injection was performed during brief (⬃60 s) isoflurane anesthesia as described previously (Stiedl et al., 2000a). ECG was continuously recorded during an 18-min period. The ECG signal (lead II) was digitized at a rate of 4 kHz and stored for later off-line analysis. Unrecognized beats in the ECG recording were edited, and artifacts were excluded from analysis. Heartbeat intervals (in milliseconds) derived from successive R-waves of the ECG signal were converted into instantaneous heart rate. For later statistical analysis, heart rate and heart rate variability were calculated in 2-min intervals. Heart rate variability was determined by the root mean square of the sum of successive RR interval differences (Stiedl and Spiess, 1997). In Situ Hybridization Histochemistry. Mice were decapitated, and the brains were rapidly dissected out and frozen. Coronal sections of the septal complex (bregma 1.00 to 0.60 mm) (Paxinos and Franklin, 2001) at 14-␮m thickness were cut using a cryostat (Dittes, Heidelberg, Germany). The sections were thaw-mounted onto pretreated glass slides (ProbeOn; Fisher Scientific Inc., Pittsburgh, PA) and stored at ⫺20°C until in situ hybridization was performed. Using MacVector software (Kodak Scientific Imaging Systems, New Haven, CT), oligonucleotide probes were selected based on optimum ratio of guanosine ⫹ cytosine/total nucleotide numbers (50 – 65%) and minimal homology (not greater than 80%) with other GenBank entered nucleotide sequences. Oligonucleotide probes complementary to nucleotides 97 to 144 of mouse 5-HT1A receptor mRNA (GenBank accession number U39391), 595 to 642 of mouse vesicular acetylcholine transporter (VAChT) mRNA (GenBank accession number NM021712), 349 to 396 of mouse calcium-binding protein parvalbumin mRNA (GenBank accession number NM013645), and 760 to 807 of mouse vesicular glutamate transporter 2 (VGLUT2) mRNA (GenBank accession number AF324864) were synthesized (CyberGene, Stockholm, Sweden). The probes were labeled with [35S]dATP (BioNuclear, Stockholm, Sweden) at the 3⬘-end using terminal deoxynucleotidyltransferase (GE Healthcare, Little Chalfont, Buckinghamshire, UK) and purified using ProbeQuant G50 micro columns (GE Healthcare). Tissue sections were hybridized overnight at 42°C with 0.5 ng of labeled probe per slide in a cocktail as described earlier (Dagerlind et al., 1992), rinsed in 1⫻ SSC buffer for 4 ⫻ 15 min at 56°C, and allowed to cool down to room temperature followed by rinsing in distilled water and dipping in 60 and 95% ethanol. After

584

Madjid et al.

air-drying, the labeled sections were apposed to ␤max autoradiography film (GE Healthcare). The films were exposed for 18 days (VAChT and parvalbumin), 4 weeks (5-HT1A receptor), or 8 weeks (VGLUT2); developed with LX 24; and fixed with AL4 (Eastman Kodak, Rochester, NY). The films were scanned (UMAX PowerLook 3000, software UMAX Magic Scan DA 4.2; UMAX Technologies, Fremont, CA) at 2000 dots per inch and processed using the Adobe Photoshop software, version 6.0 (Adobe Systems, San Jose´, CA). For control purposes, an excess (100⫻) of unlabeled probe was added to the hybridization cocktail. Statistical Analysis. The results obtained in the PA studies were analyzed using one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA). For each significant F ratio, a post hoc testing was performed using Fisher’s protected least significant different test. The overall effects of heart rate measurements were analyzed using ANOVA for repeated measures. For each significant F ratio, Fisher’s protected least significant different test was used for post hoc analysis. In the elevated plus-maze experiment, the Student-Neumann-Keuls test was used to analyze the statistical significance among multiple groups (Kirk, 1968). A probability level of p ⬍ 0.05 was accepted as statistically significant.

Results Passive Avoidance Experiments Behavioral Observations. No apparent differences in the response to the electric current were observed among the

treatment groups. The latencies to enter the dark compartment during training (training latencies) were in the range of 15 to 23 s in all of the experimental groups. The training latencies were not significantly affected by the different treatments (data not shown). This indicates that the drugs used under the present conditions did not interfere with motor control or sensory processing in a manner that resulted in an altered training latency. Pretraining Administration of 5-HT1A Receptor Ligands. When tested 24 h after training, the saline-treated NMRI control groups displayed retention latencies in the range of 60 to 100 s, indicating that the mice acquired the task. The 5-HT1A receptor agonist 8-OH-DPAT (0.01 to 1 mg/kg, 15 min prior to training) exerted a biphasic effect on PA memory retention [F(5,43) ⫽ 21.44; p ⬍ 0.01]. The lowest doses of 8-OH-DPAT (0.01 and 0.03 mg/kg) improved PA retention, indicated by an increase in the retention latencies (p ⬍ 0.01 versus saline). In contrast, the higher doses (0.1–1 mg/kg) caused a significant impairment of PA retention (p ⬍ 0.05 versus saline at 0.1 and 0.3 mg/kg; p ⬍ 0.01 versus saline at 1 mg/kg), indicated by decreased retention latencies (Fig. 1). The 5-HT1A receptor antagonist NAD-299 administered s.c. 15 min prior to training produced a dose-dependent in-

Fig. 1. The dose-response effects of 8-OH-DPAT, NAD-299, or WAY-100635 on PA retention in mice. A, 8-OH-DPAT (0.01, 0.03, 0.1, 0.3, and 1 mg/kg) (n ⫽ 6) was injected s.c. 15 min prior to training (shock intensity 0.3 mA). The saline (S) control group (n ⫽ 16) was run concurrently. B, NMRI mice were injected with WAY-100635 (0.03, 0.3, 1, 2, and 3 mg/kg) 30 min prior to training (n ⫽ 16; n ⫽ 8 in control and WAY-100635-treated groups, respectively; shock intensity 0.3 mA). The saline (S) control group was run concurrently. C, NMRI mice were injected with NAD-299 (0.1, 0.3, 1, 2, and 3 mg/kg) 15 min prior to training [n ⫽ 16; n ⫽ 8 in control (S) versus NAD-299-treated groups, respectively; shock intensity 0.3 mA]. D, C57BL/6J mice were injected with NAD-299 (0.1, 0.3, 1, and 3 mg/kg) 15 min prior to training (n ⫽ 8; shock intensity 0.2 mA). The retention test was performed 24 h after training. Vertical bars represent mean ⫾ S.E.M. of retention latency. ⴱ, p ⬍ 0.05; ⴱⴱ, p ⬍ 0.01 versus saline (S) control group.

5-HT1A Receptor Blockade Facilitates Aversive Learning

crease in PA retention in the 0.1- to 2-mg/kg dose range in NMRI mice [Fig. 1C; F(5,72) ⫽ 11.28; p ⬍ 0.001]. However, the dose-response curve of NAD-299 had an inverted U-shape (i.e., at the 3 mg/kg dose), no significant effect on PA retention was observed. The 5-HT1A receptor antagonist WAY100635 (0.03–3 mg/kg, 30 min prior to training) also produced a significant increase in PA retention [Fig. 1B; F(5,48) ⫽ 3.92; p ⬍ 0.01]. Post hoc analysis revealed a significant effect at the 1–3 mg/kg doses in NMRI mice. However, WAY-100635 at 2 and 3 mg/kg did not show the same bell-shape effect as observed with NAD-299. The C57BL/6J strain was included to compare PA performance with the result obtained in the anxiety related test (see below). A pilot experiment established that the shock sensitivity differed between the C57BL/6J and the NMRI strains. As shown in Fig. 1, C and D, C57BL/6J mice had approximately the same response latency at the retention test at a shock intensity that was 0.1 mA lower than the one used in the experiment with NMRI mice. NAD-299 (0.1–3 mg/kg, 15 min prior to training, 0.2 mA) produced a dosedependent increase in PA retention in C57BL/6J mice, with a significant facilitation of PA retention at the 1 and 3 mg/kg doses [Fig. 1D; F(4,35) ⫽ 2.96; p ⬍ 0.05]. However, the shape of the dose-response curves differed between the two strains (compare Fig. 1, C and D). Post-Training Administration of 5-HT1A Receptor Ligands and Scopolamine. This experiment was designed to investigate the putative effects of 5-HT1A receptor agonist and antagonists together with scopolamine on memory consolidation. When given immediately after training, 8-OHDPAT (0.01 and 0.3 mg/kg) (shock intensity 0.4 mA) caused no statistically significant changes in PA retention [Fig. 2A; F(2,21) ⫽ 1.67; p ⫽ 0.21, N.S.]. NAD-299 (1 mg/kg), WAY100635 (1 mg/kg), and scopolamine (0.1 mg/kg) (shock intensity, 0.4 mA) were also administered immediately post-training. However, no significant effects were found on PA retention [Fig. 2B; F(3,36) ⫽ 1.44; p ⫽ 0.25, N.S.]. Involvement of Presynaptic and Postsynaptic 5-HT1A Receptors in the Actions of 8-OH-DPAT. This experiment was aimed at investigating the 5-HT1A receptormediated mechanisms behind the biphasic effect of 8-OHDPAT on PA retention. Based on the results from the previous experiment, a facilitatory (0.03 mg/kg) and an inhibitory (1 mg/kg) dose of 8-OH-DPAT was studied in combination with NAD-299. The facilitation in PA retention induced by the low dose of 8-OH-DPAT (0.03 mg/kg) was unaffected by pretreatment with NAD-299 (Fig. 3A; N.S.). In contrast, treatment with NAD-299 (0.3 mg/kg, 30 min prior to training) completely blocked the impairment of PA retention induced by 8-OH-DPAT (1 mg/kg, 15 min prior to training) [Fig. 3B; F(4,25) ⫽ 17.42; p ⬍ 0.01]. Effects of Scopolamine and Physostigmine. To investigate possible interaction between 5-HT1A receptors and muscarinic receptors in the control of PA memory retention, initial experiments compared the effects of a muscarinic receptor antagonist (scopolamine) and an acetylcholinesterase inhibitor (physostigmine). Training latencies were not affected by scopolamine [F(3,20) ⬍ 1, N.S.] or physostigmine [F(5,33) ⬍ 1, N.S.; data not shown]. Scopolamine (0.03– 0.3 mg/kg; 40 min prior to training) markedly impaired PA retention [F(3,20) ⫽ 9.53; p ⬍ 0.01] with a significant effect from the 0.1 mg/kg dose (Fig. 4A; p ⬍ 0.01 versus saline). In

585

Fig. 2. Post-training administration of 8-OH-DPAT, NAD-299, WAY100635, and scopolamine on PA retention in mice. A, mice were injected with 8-OH-DPAT (0.01 and 0.3 mg/kg) immediate after training (shock intensity 0.4 mA). B, mice were injected with NAD-299 (1 mg/kg), WAY100635 (1 mg/kg), or scopolamine (0.1 mg/kg) immediately after training (shock intensity 0.4 mA). The saline (0) control group was run concurrently. The retention test was performed 24 h after training (n ⫽ 16; n ⫽ 8 in control and drug-treated groups, respectively). Vertical bars represent mean ⫾ S.E.M. of retention latency.

addition, the time-dependent effects of scopolamine on PA retention were examined. Notably, scopolamine (0.1 mg/kg) injected 5, 10, or 20 min prior to training produced a similar degree of impairment in PA retention [Fig. 4B; F(3,26) ⫽ 13,93; p ⬍ 0.01]. In contrast to scopolamine, physostigmine (0.0125– 0.3 mg/kg; 20 min prior to training) produced a bell-shaped increase in PA retention [Fig. 4C; F(5,33) ⫽ 8.22; p ⬍ 0.01]. Combined Effect of 5-HT1A and Muscarinic Receptor Antagonists. Based on the results from the previous experiments, the 0.03- and 0.1-mg/kg doses of scopolamine were selected for the subsequent interaction studies with NAD299. The effects of NAD-299 (0.1, 0.3, and 1 mg/kg) on the scopolamine-induced impairment of PA retention were studied using different injection schedules, with NAD-299 being injected either 10 min before or 10 min after scopolamine administration. When administered 10 min before scopolamine (0.1 mg/kg; 10 min prior to training), NAD-299 (0.1–1 mg/kg) almost completely blocked the impairment of PA retention [Fig. 5A; F(4,24) ⫽ 9.45; p ⬍ 0.01]. In contrast, when administered 10 min after scopolamine (0.1 mg/kg; 40 min prior to training), the same doses of NAD-299 (0.1–1 mg/kg) partially blocked the scopolamine-induced impairment of PA retention [Fig. 5B; F(4,34) ⫽ 26.9; p ⬍ 0.01]. On the other hand, the 0.03-mg/kg dose of scopolamine, which does not

586

Madjid et al.

Fig. 3. Effects of NAD-299 in combination with 8-OH-DPAT on PA retention in mice. A, mice were injected with NAD-299 (0.3 mg/kg s.c.) and a low dose of 8-OH-DPAT (0.03 mg/kg s.c.) 30 and 15 min prior to training, respectively (shock intensity 0.3 mA). B, mice were injected with NAD-299 (0.3 mg/kg s.c.) and a high dose of 8-OH-DPAT (1 mg/kg s.c.) 30 and 15 min prior to training, respectively (shock intensity 0.3 mA). The control groups that received two saline injections (0 ⫹ 0) were run concurrently. The retention test was performed 24 h after training. n ⫽ 6 for each treatment group. Vertical bars represent mean ⫾ S.E.M. of retention latency. ⴱⴱ, p ⬍ 0.01 versus saline ⫹ saline control group; ##, p ⬍ 0.01 versus saline ⫹ 8-OH-DPAT group.

produce any significant effect on PA retention, completely blocked the facilitatory effect of NAD-299 [Fig. 5C; F(4,25) ⫽ 15.44; p ⬍ 0.01]. Effect of MK-801 Alone or in Combination with NAD299. The NMDA receptor antagonist MK-801 (0.03– 0.3 mg/ kg; 30 min prior to training) impaired PA retention [F(2,16) ⫽ 3.57; p ⬍ 0.05] with a significant effect at the 0.3 mg/kg dose (Fig. 6A; p ⬍ 0.05 versus saline). NAD-299 (0.3 and 1 mg/kg) injected 10 min before MK-801 (0.3 mg/kg) blocked the MK-801-induced impairment of PA retention [Fig. 6B; F(3,28) ⫽ 8.98; p ⬍ 0.01]. The 0.3 mg/kg dose of MK-801, which is amnesic in the NMRI strain, did not produce any apparent ataxic effect because training latency was unaffected. Moreover, behavioral observation indicated that MK-801 at 0.3 mg/kg slightly increased motility but did not apparently affect the responsivity to the US. Effects of NAD-299 on Anxiety-Related Behavior and Autonomic Function. To investigate the potential effect of NAD-299 on the emotional state of mice, experiments were performed using the elevated plus-maze to measure anxietyrelated behavior. NAD-299 (0.3 and 1.0 mg/kg) had no significant effects on any parameter [e.g., number of arm crossings: F(2,12) ⬍ 1; N.S.; anxiety index: F(2,12) ⬍ 1; N.S.] as

Fig. 4. The dose- and time-dependent effects of scopolamine and dosedependent effects of physostigmine on PA retention in mice. A, mice were injected with scopolamine (0.03, 0.1, and 0.3 mg/kg s.c.) 40 min prior to training (n ⫽ 6; shock intensity 0.4 mA). B, mice were injected with scopolamine (0.1 mg/kg s.c.) 5, 10, and 20 min prior to training (n ⫽ 12; n ⫽ 6 in control and scopolamine-treated groups, respectively; shock intensity 0.4 mA). C, mice were injected with physostigmine (0.0125– 0.3 mg/kg) 20 min prior to training (n ⫽ 10; n ⫽ 5 in control and physostigmine-treated groups, respectively; shock intensity 0.3 mA). The saline (0) control group was run concurrently. As there were no significant differences of saline injection at different time points before training, these results were pooled. The retention test was performed 24 h after training. Vertical bars represent mean ⫾ S.E.M. of retention latency. ⴱⴱ, p ⬍ 0.01 versus saline control group.

measured in the elevated plus-maze (Fig. 7, A and B). Because emotionality is known to affect autonomic function, the effect of NAD-299 (0.3 mg/kg) on heart rate and its variability was investigated in C57BL/6J mice as an indicator of sympathetic activation under baseline stress-free conditions in the home cage. ANOVA for repeated measures did not indicate any significant differences in heart rate [F(1,15) ⬍ 1; NS] or heart rate variability [Fig. 7C; F(1,15) ⬍ 1; NS].

5-HT1A Receptor Blockade Facilitates Aversive Learning

587

Fig. 6. The dose-dependent effects of MK-801 alone or in combination with NAD-299 on PA retention in mice. A, mice were injected with MK-801 (0.03, 0.1, and 0.3 mg/kg s.c.) 30 min prior to the training session (n ⫽ 5; shock intensity 0.4 mA). The saline (0.0) control group was run concurrently. B, mice were injected with NAD-299 (0.3 and 1 mg/kg) and MK-801 (0.3 mg/kg) 30 and 20 min prior to training, respectively (n ⫽ 8; shock intensity 0.4 mA). The retention test was performed 24 h after training. Vertical bars represent mean ⫾ S.E.M. of retention latency. ⴱ, p ⬍ 0.05; ⴱⴱ, p ⬍ 0.01 versus saline control group; #, p ⬍ 0.05 and ##, p ⬍ 0.01 versus saline ⫹ MK-801-treated group.

Fig. 5. Effects of NAD-299 in combination with scopolamine on PA retention in mice. A, mice were injected with NAD-299 (0.1–1 mg/kg s.c.) and scopolamine (0.1 mg/kg s.c.) 20 and 10 min prior to training, respectively (n ⫽ 6; shock intensity 0.4 mA). B, mice were injected with scopolamine (0.1 mg/kg s.c.) and NAD-299 (0.1–1 mg/kg s.c.) 40 and 30 min prior to training (n ⫽ 10; n ⫽ 5 in control and scopolamine-treated groups, respectively; shock intensity 0.4 mA). C, mice were injected with scopolamine (0.03 mg/kg s.c.) and NAD-299 (0.3–1 mg/kg s.c.) 40 and 30 min prior to training (n ⫽ 6; shock intensity 0.4 mA). The saline ⫹ saline (0 ⫹ 0) control groups were run concurrently. The retention test was performed 24 h after training. Vertical bars represent mean ⫾ S.E.M. of retention latency. ⴱⴱ, p ⬍ 0.01 versus saline ⫹ saline control group; #, p ⬍ 0.05 and ##, p ⬍ 0.01 versus saline ⫹ scopolamine- or saline ⫹ NAD-299treated groups.

In Situ Hybridization Histochemistry. In the septal complex, weak-to-medium hybridization signal was detected in the medial septum/diagonal band of Broca (MSDB) after hybridization with an oligonucleotide probe to the 5-HT1A

receptor mRNA (Fig. 8A). Labeled cells were demonstrated in all parts of the MSDB. Strong VAChT mRNA expression was observed in the MSDB with a stronger signal in the lateral than in the medial aspect of the MSDB (Fig. 8B). Many parvalbumin-labeled cells were detected in the MSDB, mainly distributed in the medial aspect of the MSDB (Fig. 8C). Medium-strong VGLUT2 mRNA expression was observed in the medial and lateral aspects of the MSDB (Fig. 8D). No hybridization signal was observed in the septal complex after the addition of an excess of unlabeled probe (data not shown).

Discussion Because 5-HT1A receptors are localized both presynaptically and postsynaptically, the understanding of their relative role in the actions of 8-OH-DPAT is critical for the interpretation of mechanisms underlying learning. The selective 5-HT1A receptor agonist 8-OH-DPAT produced a biphasic effect on PA retention in mice probably mediated by stimulation of presynaptic and postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptors, respectively. The marked PA impairment seen at higher doses (0.1–1.0 mg/kg) of 8-OH-DPAT, which was fully blocked by NAD-299, is consistent with previous findings that stimulation of postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptors impairs

588

Madjid et al.

Fig. 8. Autoradiograms of sections of the mouse medial septum/diagonal band of Broca. A, after in situ hybridization with an oligonucleotide probe to the 5-HT1A receptor. B, the VAChT. C, the calcium-binding protein parvalbumin (PARV). D, the VGLUT2 mRNA. The 5-HT1A receptor mRNA expressing cell bodies are codistributed with cell bodies expressing VAChT, PARV, and VGLUT2 mRNA (compare A with B, C, and D, respectively). LS, lateral septum; MS, medial septum; VDB, vertical limb of the diagonal band of Broca; HDB, horizontal limb of the diagonal band of Broca. Scale bar ⫽ 500 ␮m.

Fig. 7. Effects of NAD-299 in the elevated plus-maze and on heart rate. A, the number of arm entries in the 5-min period. B, the calculated anxiety index in mice injected with NAD-299 (0.3 and 1 mg/kg) (n ⫽ 5) on the elevated plus-maze test. C, heart rate (beats per minute, bpm) and heart rate variability determined by the root-mean-square of the sum of successive RR interval differences measure (see Materials and Methods) did not differ in mice injected with saline and NAD-299 (0.3 mg/kg; n ⫽ 9). Vertical bars represent mean ⫾ S.E.M.

performance in various learning and memory tasks in rodents (see Introduction). It seems likely that the impairing effects of systemically administered 8-OH-DPAT on PA are mediated mainly but not exclusively by stimulation of hippocampal 5-HT1A receptors. Thus, administration of 8-OHDPAT into the hippocampus expressing postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptors caused deficits in aversive learning paradigms, such as PA and fear conditioning in the rat and mouse, respectively (Carli et al., 1993; Stiedl et al., 2000a). However, because both the cortex and hippocampus are involved in PA learning and retention (Santucci and Shaw, 2003), the effects of systemic 8-OH-DPAT on PA cannot be exclusively related to an action on hippocampal 5-HT1A receptors. Low doses of 8-OH-DPAT (0.01 and 0.03 mg/kg) facilitated PA retention, most likely related to stimulation of 5-HT1A autoreceptors, located on the raphe cell level (Warburton et al., 1997). This interpretation is consistent with the observation that systemic administration of low doses of 8-OH-DPAT and intraraphe administration of 8-OH-DPAT improved learning and memory in different tasks (Cole et al., 1994; Warburton et al., 1997) and reversed the learning deficit caused by intrahippocampally administered scopolamine in rats (Carli et al., 1998). The 5-HT1A receptor stimulation results in an inhibition of raphe cell firing and a decrease of 5-HT release in forebrain areas, such as the cortex and the hippocampus. In turn, this results in a decrease in the basal activity at all 5-HT receptors innervated by the 5-HT terminal system, including the 5-HT1A receptor. The decrease in tonic activity at postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptors after 8-OHDPAT administration will in principle be the same as the decrease in tonic activity caused by a postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptor blockade. It must be emphasized that, unlike 8-OH-

5-HT1A Receptor Blockade Facilitates Aversive Learning

DPAT, the facilitatory action of NAD-299 on PA performance is related to its postsynaptic blocking action, either at cell body levels in the septum and/or nucleus basalis or at hippocampal/cortical neurons. Although NAD-299 certainly binds to the 5-HT1A autoreceptors and blocks its action, the compound also blocks the tonic inhibitory action of 5-HT at postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptors in hippocampal and/or cortical cells. This explains why NAD-299 cannot block the procognitive effect of 8-OH-DPAT. This interpretation is in line with the observation that, when NAD-299 and a low procognitive dose of 8-OH-DPAT are combined, there was no enhancement of PA retention performance compared with the effect caused by each compound separately. These results suggest that there exists an optimal dynamic range in which reduction in tonic 5-HT neurotransmission can improve learning performance. A problem in the analysis of the role of the 5-HT1A receptor in cognition is that most of the evidence is based on results obtained with 8-OH-DPAT, which has about a 10 times lower affinity for the 5-HT7 receptor than it has for the 5-HT1A receptor (Hoyer et al., 1994). Using nonselective 5-HT receptor antagonists, the contribution of the 5-HT7 receptors in the impairing actions of 8-OH-DPAT in the rat seems to be negligible. Thus, methiothepin and spiperone, with much higher affinities for the 5-HT7 receptor than the 5-HT1A receptor, failed to block the PA impairment induced by the 0.2-mg/kg ¨ gren, 2000). Moredose of 8-OH-DPAT in the rat (Misane and O over, the impairment of PA by 8-OH-DPAT is fully blocked by pretreatment with the selective 5-HT1A antagonists NAD-299 and WAY-100635 as shown in this and previous studies (Mis¨ gren, 2000; Lu¨ttgen et al., 2005a). These findings ane and O indicate that it is not likely that 8-OH-DPAT mediates its effect on PA via stimulation of 5-HT7 receptors. The physiological role of 5-HT1A receptors in rodent cognition has remained unclear due to the inconsistent results reported after various 5-HT1A receptor antagonists (see Introduction). However, the present results indicate that systemic administration of selective 5-HT1A receptor antagonists can facilitate PA retention. This suggests that 5-HT1A receptors of importance for cognitive function are tonically activated by endogenous 5-HT, contributing to a suboptimal PA performance. On the other hand, the impaired spatial learning observed in 5-HT1A receptor knockout mice (Sarnyai et al., 2000) is difficult to reconcile with the present findings. This discrepancy could be explained by compensatory changes in 5-HT-related transmission in the 5-HT1A receptor knockout mice (Ramboz et al., 1998). Although approximately half as potent as a 5-HT1A receptor antagonist (Ki: NAD-299, 0.59 nM; WAY-100635, 0.24 nM) (Johansson et al., 1997), NAD-299 was more effective in its facilitatory action than WAY-100635. The facilitatory effect of NAD-299 reached a somewhat greater magnitude than that of the acetylcholinesterase inhibitor physostigmine. This may be due to the fact that NAD-299 is a more selective antagonist than WAY-100635, with a low affinity for both adrenergic and dopaminergic receptors (Johansson et al., 1997). Interestingly, both NAD-299 and physostigmine displayed a bell-shaped dose-response curve in the NMRI strain, which probably is related to the appearance of peripheral muscarinic side effects (tremor and salivation) and/or central cholinergic overstimulation (Yoshida and Suzuki, 1993). NAD-299 administration did not produce any muscarinic side effects, but it is possible that NAD-299 at higher doses

589

enhances 5-HT transmission and/or stimulates 5-HT1A re¨ gren, 2003). ceptors, resulting in impairment (Misane and O In this respect, NAD-299 and the “silent” 5-HT1A receptor antagonist WAY-100635 may differ, because the dose-response curve of the latter did not display an inverted U shape (Fletcher et al., 1996). The role of 5-HT1A receptors in associative learning tasks has been mainly related to the acquisition or encoding phase ¨ gren, 1985; of learning and not to the consolidation phase (O Misane et al., 1998; Stiedl et al., 2000b). Thus, immediate post-training administration of 5-HT1A receptor agonists failed to significantly alter PA retention or fear conditioning (Misane et al., 1998; Stiedl et al., 2000b). The present study failed to reveal any significant changes of PA retention after immediate post-training administration of the 5-HT1A receptor antagonists and 8-OH-DPAT. In contrast, the 5-HT1A receptor antagonist NAN-190 (1 mg/kg) was reported to facilitate PA retention when administered immediately posttraining (Schneider et al., 2003). These contradictory findings indicate that more decisive studies are needed to elucidate whether the 5-HT1A receptor has a significant role in memory consolidation. Besides encoding, there is evidence that 5-HT1A receptors may play a role in retrieval of aversive information, because 8-OH-DPAT injected prior to the retention test impaired PA memory retention in the rat (Misane et al., 1998). Because the compounds were administered prior to training, the results obtained could be due to changes in noncognitive processes, such as sensorimotor function and changes in the emotional state of the animals. However, neither NAD299 nor 8-OH-DPAT alone nor their combination influenced training latency or the responsivity to the electric current in a significant manner. Moreover, NAD-299 given at doses that facilitated PA retention did not influence anxiety-related behavior. Moreover, autonomic responses seem not to be altered after 5-HT1A receptor blockade tested in the C57BL/6J strain, which is known to be a highly emotional strain (Stiedl et al., 1999; Griebel et al., 2000). An anxiety-like state is expected to enhance sympathetic activity when compared with stress-free conditions (Berntson et al., 1998). Because the heart rate and its variability were unaffected, the results did not indicate alterations in anxiety states by 5-HT1A receptor blockade. The failure of NAD-299 to change the emotional state is unexpected, because studies with different strains of 5-HT1A receptor knockout mice reported enhanced anxiety as measured in various anxiety tests (Ramboz et al., 1998). Thus, the modulatory effects of the 5-HT1A receptor antagonist on cognitive functions in mice seem not to be directly related to changes in emotional state (i.e., increased anxiety) or autonomic functions. There is evidence that 5-HT exerts a modulatory effect on cognition through interactions with the cholinergic system (Cassel and Jeltsch, 1995; Steckler and Sahgal, 1995; Millan et al., 2004). The present results support this hypothesis because blockade of 5-HT1A receptors prevented the memory deficit caused by blockade of cholinergic muscarinic receptors by scopolamine. Scopolamine is known to induce memory deficits in humans as well as in rodents (Blokland, 1996). Furthermore, our results are in agreement with the finding that both WAY-100635 and NAD-299 attenuated the PA impairment caused by scopolamine when injected before ¨ gren, 2003). and/or after scopolamine in rats (Misane and O Similar to the results with WAY-100635 and NAD-299 ob-

590

Madjid et al.

¨ gren, 2003), NAD-299 was more tained in rats (Misane and O effective when injected 10 min prior to scopolamine than 10 min afterward. This suggests that the temporal kinetics of 5-HT1A receptor blockade in relationship to muscarinic blockade by scopolamine is important, as noted previously in the ¨ gren, 2003). rat (Misane and O The ability of 5-HT1A receptor antagonists to block or attenuate the memory impairments by scopolamine or MK-801 probably involves multiple mechanisms. An immunohistochemical study in the rat provided evidence that 5-HT1A receptors are colocalized with choline acetyltransferase, a marker for cholinergic neurons, in the MSDB (Kia et al., 1996; Lu¨ttgen et al., 2005b). Because the cellular localization of the 5-HT1A receptor in the mouse is not well known, the localization of 5-HT1A receptor mRNA expressing cell bodies was investigated in the mouse MSDB, the origin of the cholinergic input to the hippocampal formation (Kiss et al., 1990; Bland and Oddie, 1998). The 5-HT1A receptor protein has been reported to have a somatodendritic localization (Kia et al., 1996); therefore, 5-HT1A receptor mRNA can be used to identify neurons possessing 5-HT1A receptors. In the MSDB, cell bodies expressing 5-HT1A receptor mRNA were found to be codistributed with cell bodies expressing VAChT (a marker for cholinergic neurons) (Roghani et al., 1994), parvalbumin (a marker for septal GABAergic neurons projecting to the hippocampus) (Freund, 1989), and VGLUT2 (a marker for glutamatergic neurons) (Lin et al., 2003). The present anatomical data suggest that 5-HT1A receptor transmission can influence cholinergic, GABAergic, and glutamatergic transmissions in the medial septal area. The MSDB and nucleus basalis magnocellularis neurons provide the main cholinergic innervation of the hippocampus and cerebral cortex, respectively. Stimulation of 5-HT1A receptors is known to reduce the excitability of cholinergic and glutamatergic neurons (see below). This suggests that blockade of tonic 5-HT1A receptor transmission in the basal forebrain might result in a disinhibition of cholinergic septal and/or nucleus basalis magnocellularis neurons, resulting in enhancement of hippocampal/cortical cholinergic transmission. This hypothesis is supported by a recent in vivo microdialysis study showing that NAD-299 and WAY-100635 significantly increased basal ACh release in the hippocampus and cortex of freely moving rats in the dose range that displayed procognitive effects and blocked cognitive deficits induced by scopolamine (Hu et al., 2003; Millan et al., 2004). Moreover, recent studies with the selective antagonist lecozotan showed an enhancement of evoked release of ACh and glutamate in the hippocampus in the rat at doses that enhanced cognitive functions (Schechter et al., 2005). The importance of enhanced ACh transmission for the action of 5-HT1A receptor blockade is further supported by the observation that a subthreshold dose of scopolamine (0.03 mg/kg) completely antagonized the facilitatory effect of NAD-299 on PA retention. The PA retention deficit caused by pretraining administration of MK-801 is consistent with a number of studies showing that NMDA receptors are important in the encoding phase of aversive learning (Parada-Turska and Turski, 1990; Stiedl et al., 2000a). Notably, NAD-299 attenuated the actions of MK-801 in the same dose range in which it blocked the impairments by scopolamine. Consistently, WAY-100635 attenuated the cognitive impairment induced by both MK-

801 treatment and cholinergic dysfunctions (fornix transection) in the marmoset (Harder and Ridley, 2000). In addition to enhancement of ACh and glutamate release, another site for the 5-HT1A and muscarinic/glutamatergic interactions might involve cortical/hippocampal pyramidal neurons. Both 5-HT1A and muscarinic receptors are localized on pyramidal hippocampal cells (Azmitia et al., 1996; van der Zee and Luiten, 1999). 5-HT1A receptor stimulation hyperpolarizes pyramidal neurons (Tada et al., 1999), whereas increases in ACh stimulates excitatory transmission (Dutar et al., 1995). Hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons are tonically inhibited by endogenous 5-HT through postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptors during conditioned fear learning (Tada et al., 2004). By blocking the hyperpolarization action of endogenous 5-HT mediated by 5-HT1A receptors, 5-HT1A receptor antagonists could compensate for the reduction of cholinergic afferent input and NMDA receptor-mediated excitatory drive on pyramidal cells in the hippocampus/cortex. A similar mechanism may explain that the PA impairment caused by the NMDA receptor antagonist MK-801 is blocked by NAD-299. Thus, 5-HT1A receptor blockade can counteract the tonic inhibition of glutamatergic pyramidal cells after activation of 5-HT1A receptors. In summary, the present results suggest that 5-HT1A receptor antagonists can enhance cognitive functions by facilitating cholinergic and/or glutamatergic transmissions in the hippocampus/cortex, in line with earlier proposals (Bowen et al., 1994). Studies in healthy human subjects using positron emission tomography with [11C]WAY-100635 also support a role for hippocampal 5-HT1A receptors in explicit memory (Yasuno et al., 2003). These findings indicate that 5-HT1A receptor antagonists may be beneficial in the treatment of cognitive loss in disorders characterized by cholinergic and glutamatergic dysfunctions, such as Alzheimer’s disease and age-related memory disorders. Acknowledgments

We gratefully acknowledge the experimental work performed by Asadi-Kenari Fatemeh. We thank Carina Stenfors (AstraZeneca R&D, So¨derta¨lje, Sweden) for the supply of NAD-299. References Aghajanian GK (1995) Electrophysiology of serotonin receptor subtypes and signal transduction pathways, in Psychopharmacology: The Fourth Generation of Progress (Bloom FE and Kupfer DJ eds) pp 451– 459, Raven Press, New York. Azmitia EC, Gannon PJ, Kheck NM, and Whitaker-Azmitia PM (1996) Cellular localization of the 5-HT1A receptor in primate brain neurons and glial cells. Neuropsychopharmacology 14:35– 46. Belcheva I, Belcheva S, Petkov VV, Hadjiivanova C, and Petkov VD (1997) Behavorial responses to the 5-HT1A receptor antagonist NAN190 injected into rat CA1 hippocampal area. Gen Pharmacol 28:435– 441. Berntson GG, Sarter M, and Cacioppo JT (1998) Anxiety and cardiovascular reactivity: the basal forebrain cholinergic link. Behav Brain Res 94:225–248. Bland BH and Oddie SD (1998) Anatomical, electrophysiological and pharmacological studies of ascending brain stem hippocampal synchronizing pathways. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 22:259 –273. Blokland A (1996) Acetylcholine: a neurotransmitter for learning and memory? Brain Res Rev 21:285–300. Bowen DM, Francis PT, Chessell IP, and Webster MT (1994) Neurotransmission–the link integrating Alzheimer research? Trends Neurosci 17:149 –150. Buhot MC, Martin S, and Segu L (2000) Role of serotonin in memory impairment. Ann Med 32:210 –221. Carli M, Bonalumi P, and Samanin R (1997) WAY 100635, a 5-HT1A receptor antagonist, prevents the impairment of spatial learning caused by intrahippocampal administration of scopolamine or 7-chloro-kynurenic acid. Brain Res 774:167– 174. Carli M, Bonalumi P, and Samanin R (1998) Stimulation of 5-HT1A receptors in the dorsal raphe reverses the impairment of spatial learning caused by intrahippocampal scopolamine in rats. Eur J Neurosci 10:221–230. Carli M, Lazarova M, Tatarczynska E, and Samanin R (1992) Stimulation of 5-HT1A receptors in the dorsal hippocampus impairs acquisition and performance of a spatial task in a water maze. Brain Res 595:50 –56. Carli M, Luschi R, Garofalo P, and Samanin R (1995) 8-OH-DPAT impairs spatial

5-HT1A Receptor Blockade Facilitates Aversive Learning but not visual learning in a water maze by stimulating 5-HT1A receptors in the hippocampus. Behav Brain Res 67:67–74. Carli M, Tatarczynska E, Cervo L, and Samanin R (1993) Stimulation of hippocampal 5-HT1A receptors causes amnesia and anxiolytic-like but not antidepressantlike effects in the rat. Eur J Pharmacol 234:215–221. Cassel JC and Jeltsch H (1995) Serotonergic modulation of cholinergic function in the central nervous system: cognitive implications. Neuroscience 69:1– 41. Chalmers DT and Watson SJ (1991) Comparative anatomical distribution of 5-HT1A receptor mRNA and 5-HT1A binding in rat brain–a combined in situ hybridisation/in vitro receptor autoradiographic study. Brain Res 561:51– 60. Cole BJ, Jones GH, and Turner JD (1994) 5-HT1A receptor agonists improve the performance of normal and scopolamine-impaired rats in an operant delayed matching to position task. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 116:135–142. ˙ , Friberg K, Bean AJ, and Ho¨kfelt T (1992) Sensitive mRNA detection Dagerlind A using unfixed tissue: combined radioactive and non-radioactive in situ hybridization histochemistry. Histochemistry 98:39 – 49. Dahlstro¨m A and Fuxe K (1964) Evidence for the existence of monoamine neurons in the central nervous system. I. Demonstration of monoamines in the cell bodies of brainstem neurons. Acta Physiol Scand 62:1–55. Dutar P, Bassant MH, Senut MC, and Lamour Y (1995) The septohippocampal pathway: structure and function of a central cholinergic system. Physiol Rev 75:393– 427. Fletcher A, Forster EA, Bill DJ, Brown G, Cliffe IA, Hartley JE, Jones DE, McLenachan A, Stanhope KJ, Critchley DJ, et al. (1996) Electrophysiological, biochemical, neurohormonal and behavioral studies with WAY-100635, a potent, selective and silent 5-HT1A receptor antagonist. Behav Brain Res 73:337–353. Foreman MM, Fuller RW, Leander JD, Benvenga MJ, Wong DT, Nelson DL, Calligaro DO, Swanson SP, Lucot JB, and Flaugh ME (1993) Preclinical studies on LY228729: a potent and selective serotonin1A agonist. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 267:58 –71. Freund TF (1989) GABAergic septohippocampal neurons contain parvalbumin. Brain Res 478:375–381. Galeotti N, Ghelardini C, and Bartolini A (2000) Role of 5-HT1A receptors in a mouse passive avoidance paradigm. Jpn J Pharmacol 84:418 – 424. Griebel G, Belzung C, Perrault G, and Sanger DJ (2000) Differences in anxietyrelated behaviours and in sensitivity to diazepam in inbred and outbred strains of mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 148:164 –170. Harder JA and Ridley RM (2000) The 5-HT1A antagonist, WAY 100635, alleviates cognitive impairments induced by dizocilpine (MK-801) in monkeys. Neuropharmacology 39:547–552. Hoyer D, Clarke DE, Fozard JR, Hartig PR, Martin GR, Mylecharane EJ, Saxena PR, and Humphrey PP (1994) International Union of Pharmacology classification of receptors for 5-hydroxytryptamine (Serotonin). Pharmacol Rev 46:157–203. Hoyer D, Hannon JP, and Martin GR (2002) Molecular, pharmacological and functional diversity of 5-HT receptors. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 71:533–554. Hu XJ, Wang FH, Osborne PG, Ogren SO, and Kehr J (2003) Effects of the 5-HT1A receptor antagonist robalzotan (NAD-299) on extracellular acetylcholine levels in the frontal cortex of awake rats: A microdialysis study, in Monitoring Molecules in Neuroscience (Kehr J, Fuxe K, Ungerstedt U, and Svensson T eds), pp 354 –356, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden. Johansson L, Sohn D, Thorberg SO, Jackson DM, Kelder D, Larsson LG, Re´nyi L, Ross SB, Wallsten C, Eriksson H, et al. (1997) The pharmacological characterization of a novel selective 5-hydroxytryptamine1A receptor antagonist, NAD-299. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 283:216 –225. Kant GJ, Wylie RM, Chu K, and Ghosh S (1998) Effects of the serotonin agonists 8-OH-DPAT, buspirone and DOI on water maze performance. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 59:729 –735. Kia HK, Brisorgueil MJ, Daval G, Langlois X, Hamon M, and Verge´ D (1996) Serotonin1A receptors are expressed by a subpopulation of cholinergic neurons in the rat medial septum and diagonal band of Broca–a double immunohistochemical study. Neuroscience 74:143–154. Kirk RE (1968) Experimental Design: Procedures for the Behavioural Sciences. Brooks/Cole Publishing Company, Thomson Learning, Florence, KY. Kiss J, Patel AJ, and Freund TF (1990) Distribution of septohippocampal neurons containing parvalbumin or choline acetyltransferase in the rat brain. J Comp Neurol 298:362–372. Lin W, McKinney K, Liu L, Lakhlani S, and Jennes L (2003) Distribution of vesicular glutamate transporter-2 messenger ribonucleic acid and protein in the septumhypothalamus of the rat. Endocrinology 144:662– 670. ¨ gren SO (2005a) Analysis of the role of Lu¨ttgen M, Elvander E, Madjid N, and O 5-HT1A receptors in spatial and aversive learning in the rat. Neuropharmacology 48:830 – 852. ¨ gren SO, and Meister B (2005b) 5-HT1A receptor mRNA and immuLu¨ttgen M, O noreactivity in the rat medial septum/diagonal band of Broca-relationships to GABAergic and cholinergic neurons. J Chem Neuroanat 29:93–111. Manuel-Apolinar L and Meneses A (2004) 8-OH-DPAT facilitated memory consolidation and increased hippocampal and cortical cAMP production. Behav Brain Res 148:179 –184. Mendelson SD, Quartermain D, Francisco T, and Shemer A (1993) 5-HT1A receptor agonists induce anterograde amnesia in mice through a postsynaptic mechanism. Eur J Pharmacol 236:177–182. Millan MJ, Gobert A, Roux S, Porsolt R, Meneses A, Carli M, Di Cara B, Jaffard R, Rivet JM, Lestage P, et al. (2004) The serotonin1A receptor partial agonist S15535 [4-(benzodioxan-5-yl)1-(indan-2-yl)piperazine] enhances cholinergic transmission and cognitive function in rodents: a combined neurochemical and behavioral analysis. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 311:190 –203. ¨ gren SO (1998) Analysis of the 5-HT1A receptor Misane I, Johansson C, and O involvement in passive avoidance in the rat. Br J Pharmacol 125:499 –509. ¨ gren SO (2000) Multiple 5-HT receptors in passive avoidance: comMisane I and O parative studies of p-chloroamphetamine and 8-OH-DPAT. Neuropsychopharmacology 22:168 –190.

591

¨ gren SO (2003) Selective 5-HT1A antagonists WAY 100635 and Misane I and O NAD-299 attenuate the impairment of passive avoidance caused by scopolamine in the rat. Neuropsychopharmacology 28:253–264. ¨ gren SO (1985) Evidence for a role of brain serotonergic neurotransmission in O avoidance learning. Acta Physiol Scand 125:1–71. ¨ gren SO (1986) Analysis of the avoidance learning deficit induced by the serotonin O releasing compound p-chloroamphetamine. Brain Res Bull 16:645– 660. Parada-Turska J and Turski WA (1990) Excitatory amino acid antagonists and memory: effect of drugs acting at N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors in learning and memory tasks. Neuropharmacology 29:1111–1116. Paxinos G and Franklin KBJ (2001) The Mouse Brain Atlas in Stereotaxic Coordinates. Academic Press, Inc., New York. Pitsikas N, Rigamonti AE, Cella SG, and Muller EE (2003) The 5-HT 1A receptor antagonist WAY 100635 improves rats performance in different models of amnesia evaluated by the object recognition task. Brain Res 983:215–222. Pompeiano M, Palacios JM, and Mengod G (1992) Distribution and cellular localization of mRNA coding for 5-HT1A receptor in the rat brain: correlation with receptor binding. J Neurosci 12:440 – 453. Ramboz S, Oosting R, Amara DA, Kung HF, Blier P, Mendelsohn M, Mann JJ, Brunner D, and Hen R (1998) Serotonin receptor 1A knockout: an animal model of anxiety-related disorder. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95:14476 –14481. Roghani A, Feldman J, Kohan SA, Shirzadi A, Gundersen CB, Brecha N, and Edwards RH (1994) Molecular cloning of a putative vesicular transporter for acetylcholine. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 91:10620 –10624. Rowan MJ, Cullen WK, and Moulton B (1990) Buspirone impairment of performance of passive avoidance and spatial learning tasks in the rat. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 100:393–398. Sanger DJ and Joly D (1989) Performance of a passive avoidance response is disrupted by compounds acting at 5HT(1A) receptors. Behav Pharmacol 1:235–240. Santucci AC and Shaw C (2003) Peripheral 8-OH-DPAT and scopolamine infused into the frontal cortex produce passive avoidance retention impairments in rats. Neurobiol Learn Mem 79:136 –141. Sarnyai Z, Sibille EL, Pavlides C, Fenster RJ, McEwen BS, and Toth M (2000) Impaired hippocampal-dependent learning and functional abnormalities in the hippocampus in mice lacking serotonin(1A) receptors. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 97:14731–14736. Schechter LE, Smith DL, Rosenzweig-Lipson S, Sukoff SJ, Dawson LA, Marquis K, Jones D, Piesla M, Andree T, Nawoschik S, et al. (2005) Lecozotan (SRA-333): a selective serotonin 1A receptor antagonist that enhances the stimulated release of glutamate and acetylcholine in the hippocampus and possesses cognitiveenhancing properties. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 314:1274 –1289. Schneider AM, Wilkins E, Firestone A, Everbach EC, Naylor JC, and Simson PE (2003) Enhanced retention in the passive-avoidance task by 5-HT(1A) receptor blockade is not associated with increased activity of the central nucleus of the amygdala. Learn Mem 10:394 – 400. Sprouse JS and Aghajanian GK (1987) Electrophysiological responses of serotonergic dorsal raphe neurons to 5-HT1A and 5-HT1B agonists. Synapse 1:3–9. Steckler T and Sahgal A (1995) The role of serotonergic-cholinergic interactions in the mediation of cognitive behaviour. Behav Brain Res 67:165–199. Stiedl O, Birkenfeld K, Palve M, and Spiess J (2000a) Impairment of conditioned contextual fear of C57BL/6J mice by intracerebral injections of the NMDA receptor antagonist APV. Behav Brain Res 116:157–168. ¨ gren SO (2000b) Involvement of the 5-HT1A receptors Stiedl O, Misane I, Spiess J, and O in classical fear conditioning in C57BL/6J mice. J Neurosci 20:8515– 8527. Stiedl O, Radulovic J, Lohmann R, Birkenfeld K, Palve M, Kammermeier J, Sananbenesi F, and Spiess J (1999) Strain and substrain differences in context- and tone-dependent fear conditioning of inbred mice. Behav Brain Res 104:1–12. Stiedl O and Spiess J (1997) Effect of tone-dependent fear conditioning on heart rate and behavior of C57BL/6N mice. Behav Neurosci 111:703–711. Tada K, Kasamo K, Suzuki T, Matsuzaki Y, and Kojima T (2004) Endogenous 5-HT inhibits firing activity of hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons during conditioned fear stress-induced freezing behavior through stimulating 5-HT1A receptors. Hippocampus 14:143–147. Tada K, Kasamo K, Ueda N, Suzuki T, Kojima T, and Ishikawa K (1999) Anxiolytic 5-hydroxytryptamine1A agonists suppress firing activity of dorsal hippocampus CA1 pyramidal neurons through a postsynaptic mechanism: single-unit study in unanesthetized, unrestrained rats. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 288:843– 848. van der Zee EA and Luiten PG (1999) Muscarinic acetylcholine receptors in the hippocampus, neocortex and amygdala: a review of immunocytochemical localization in relation to learning and memory. Prog Neurobiol 58:409 – 471. Warburton EC, Harrison AA, Robbins TW, and Everitt BJ (1997) Contrasting effects of systemic and intracerebral infusions of the 5-HT1A receptor agonist 8-OH-DPAT on spatial short-term working memory in rats. Behav Brain Res 84:247–258. Winsauer PJ, Rodriguez FH, Cha AE, and Moerschbaecher JM (1999) Full and partial 5-HT1A receptor agonists disrupt learning and performance in rats. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 288:335–347. Winter JC and Petti DT (1987) The effects of 8-hydroxy-2-(di-n-propylamino)tetralin and other serotonergic agonists on performance in a radial maze: a possible role for 5-HT1A receptors in memory. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 27:625– 628. Yasuno F, Suhara T, Nakayama T, Ichimiya T, Okubo Y, Takano A, Ando T, Inoue M, Maeda J, and Suzuki K (2003) Inhibitory effect of hippocampal 5-HT1A receptors on human explicit memory. Am J Psychiatry 160:334 –340. Yoshida S and Suzuki N (1993) Antiamnesic and cholinomimetic side-effects of the cholinesterase inhibitors, physostigmine, tacrine and NIK-247 in rats. Eur J Pharmacol 250:117–124.

¨ gren, Dept of Neuroscience, Div Address correspondence to: Dr. Sven Ove O of Behavioral Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, Retzius va¨g 8, SE-171 77 Stockholm, Sweden. E-mail: [email protected]