Caloric, but not macronutrient, compensation by ...

3 downloads 0 Views 3MB Size Report
Bran,. Rice Krispies, and Special-K,. Kellog's,. Battle. Creek,. MI; green pepper steak,. Lean. Cuisine,. Stouffer,. Solon,. OH; spaghetti with meat sauce,. Healthy.
Caloric, but not macronutrient, compensation by humans for required-eating occasions with meals and snack varying in fat and carbohydrate13 Richard W Foltin, Barbara J Rolls, Timothy Amy L McNelis, and Marian W Fischman ABSTRACT assessing

ulations during and afternoon Overall,

participated

ofcovert

macronutrient

three snack)

energy

7000

compensated

and

required-eating occasions on total macronutrient

content

ofthe

kJ (o700

the differential Each condition low CHO; and

in a residential

and

occasions

varied

1700

with

kcal)

study

energy

content

of required

occasions

(breakfast, and energy

lunch, intakes.

effects

o3000

postulate

between majority

required

(high-CHO

condition:

evidence

ential

effects

The

± 2686

Id).

compensation,

ofcondition

no macronutrient

1 356

occasions

U).

± 3314 in the

Only

In

total

contrast

there

on macronutrient

were

intake,

compensation.

Am

J Clin

to the

ie, there

was

feeding

WORDS

Fat, carbohydrate,

compensation,

humans,

caloric

intake,

food

preloads

(6-9). slow

food

with

the

effects

Recently,

a study

even when by > 1674

meals

have

subjects

intake

from

researchers have

example,

to

different

because

of its

emptying,

higher

fat has been proposed CHO, eg, joule-for-joule than

in

mixed

this

will CHO

differential

fat

laboratory

to fat

loads

Subjects of foods and

CHO

with

(10).

contents

respect

to this

demonstrated stable

daily

that energy

ofa mandatory lunch Caloric compensation was derived

was little

and

results

a relatively

the energy intake Id (400 kcal) (18). there

postabsorptive

led

of gastric

more

varying

yielded

maintained

the energy

ma-

the diverse theoretical arguments pertaining regulation ( 1 1 - 1 3), short-term studies assess-

of

on food choice issue (14-17).

and have

For rate

preloads

reduce

of low-fat

macronutrients

slow absorption, efficacy than

compensation. wide range

intake,

these

effects,

will

success

(CHO)

behavior

In accordance to macronutrient

the

in the preabsorptive

carbohydrate

of osmotic

Furthermore, KEY

limit

energy density, and have higher satiating

when

331-42.

differences

would

that joule-for-joule, on

man

1992;55:

of items

of fat and

absence

ing

no differ-

Nutr

large

effects

of

condition ( 12 326 ± 2548 kJ) its matched CHO condition

14 665

for caloric

±

fat content

nipulations.

the

of the

(1 3 297

energy intake under the high-fat was significantly different from clear

the

manip-

derived from either fat or carbohydrate (CHO). (high, medium, and low fat; high, medium, and no required eating) was examined for 2 d. Subjects for the

H Kelly,

energy

such that only under the low-CHO condition (1 1 297 Id) was total daily energy intake lower than that observed absence

Thomas

evidence

from

either

hu-

intake

meal varied was similar fat or CHO.

of macronutrient-specific

had relatively unrestricted access to a the study of food intake was embedded

in the measurement of many behaviors (eg, work performance, social activity), so that the principal interest in food intake was

Introduction In accordance ciation

between

obesity

and

with

cancer

recommending daily addition,

the

of fat intake

from

growing

experts

of a positive disease,

to the

and

in the United

of fat be reduced the

public

led

evidence coronary

nutrition

intake

intake has

growing

fat and

(1), that

energy

the

dietary

current concern

about

development

asso-

States

are

of total

of 35-37%

(2).

the

effects

adverse

of many

ical and

between

to zo30%

total

not readily apparent. Under similar living conditions in other previous research, subjects were sensitive to both pharmacolog-

In

increase for the

from

sources

dieting intake content

several

individuals even when

their intake of fat from other sources to reduction due to the low-fat items. Data indicate maintain provided

(4, 5). Similar

specific

that

under

certain

a relatively with some

conditions

consistent total foods of lower

fat compensation in USA.

creased

MW amines

manipulations

smoked food

intake

Fischman,

that

marijuana, (4,

19, 20; TH

unpublished

decreased

food

and

intake

affect

eating

diazepam Kelly,

observations,

behavior: taken

RW

Foltin,

199 1) and

caloric orally

in-

L King,

amphet-

(21).

commercial

products proclaiming reduced fat, cholesterol, and/or low-density-lipoprotein contents (3). A question that arises from the availability of such products is whether, when presented with numerous low-fat foods, individuals will reduce their fat intake or will they compensate

energy

dilution,

nonenergy energy

for changes © 1992 American

in Society

I From the Division ofBehavioral Biology, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, The Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore. 2 Supported by National Institute ofDiabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases grant 39 177 (to BJR). 3 Address reprint requests to RW Foltin, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, The Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, 600 North Wolfe Street, Houck E-2, Baltimore, MD 21205. Received February 1 1, 1991. Accepted for publication May 16, 1991.

for Clinical

Nutrition

331

Downloaded from www.ajcn.org by guest on April 26, 2012

and

Six subjects the effects

H Moran,

FOLTIN

332 The present experiment further relative influence of caloric and

investigated macronutrient

normal-weight

no history

residing

male

subjects,

continuously

observation signed

of human

to test

regulation

the

(1674

study that

the

restraint,

for the long-term

Specifically, that

1(J), and

ofdietary

designed

behavior.

hypothesis

in the previous

differential

with

in a laboratory

the issue of the regulation in

this

absence

study

was

(18) was due to the small with

de-

of macronutrient

a larger

energy

differential

there

would be evidence for macronutrient the maximum differential between with this differential being derived

regulation. In this study conditions was > 4000 Id, predominantly from fat or CHO. As in the earlier study (18), the foods used in this research were commercially available and subjects had experience with them. Thus, the research addressed fat and CHO intakes in the general sense and did not investigate possible differences among various fats or CHOs.

Subjects

and methods

were

paid

for participation.

that they were participating performance stability over nutrition would

They

in a study prolonged

occasions

Johns

Hopkins

each

day.

This

Joint

Committee

in groups

of three,

were

instructed

on the effects of computerperiods of time. Because

can affect performance, they be given food to be consumed

eating

were also told during three

research

was

on Clinical

that they required-

approved

by The

Investigation.

Laboratory Subjects, designed

for

continuous

lived

observation

in a residential of human

laboratory behavior

over

extended periods oftime (25). The facility consisted ofsix rooms connected by a common corridor and was housed within a wing of The Johns Hopkins Hospital. Three identical private rooms were similar to small efficiency apartments with a kitchen, a bathroom, desks, and sleeping areas. The common social area had a recreation room, an exercise room, and a bathroom. The recreation room contained kitchen facilities, lounge furniture, a variety cassette

of games, recorder

puzzles, with

a video

monitor. scale with

tamed an electronic the daily weighing

of subjects.

ercise

(stationary

equipment

The

game

system,

recreation

and room

a videoalso

read-out in the control room The exercise room contained bicycle,

free-weights,

etc.)

con-

for cxand

laundry facilities. Two-way cabinets in each room of the laboratory allowed the transfer of items between subjects and cxperimenters without direct contact. Each private room had an Apple lIe microcomputer (Cupertino, CA) located on the subject’s

desk,

and

room

of the

social

Output from a video and audio monitoring system terminated in an adjacent control room. Subjects were observed continuously except when they were in private dressing areas and toilet facilities. Communication between subjects and experimenters was kept to a minimum and was accomplished by using a networked computer system consisting of the computers in each room of the laboratory and in the main control room. This networked

a similar area.

computer

was

located

in the

main

communication

system

allowed

for continuous

on-

line interaction between subjects and experimenters. Once a day, however, a staff member met with each subject individually to discuss issues related to study participation. Subjects were provided a newspaper each morning and had access to a telephone for making local calls during the evening social-access period. Standard

day

Subjects were awakened at 0900 by a tone that sounded until the subject signaled that he was awake, and the day ended with lights out at 2400. Subjects were weighed each morning in stockfeet

after

voiding.

There

were

two

3-h

work

periods

each

day: 1000- 1 300 and I 330- 1630. During these periods subjects were instructed to remain in their private rooms and engage in one of four computerized work tasks. Subjects selected a task from a list ofoptions presented on each private-room computer monitor. The other options available during these periods were engaging

in eating

or toileting

activities.

Work

tasks

lasted

3

mm

and subjects were given a small bonus at the end of the study on the basis of their performance during each work period (one to four dollars per period, or two to eight dollars per day). At 0930 subjects were provided with a breakfast meal in their private rooms and were given 30 mm to consume the entire meal, and at 1300 subjects were provided with a lunch meal in their private rooms and given 30 mm to consume the entire meal. A final required-eating occasion occurred at 1630, when subjects were given a snack to be consumed within 1 5 mm. A staff member visited each subject individually in his private room at 1645. Beginning at 1700, subjects had access to the activities in the social rooms, ie, a social-access period. Two videotaped films were shown beginning at 1800 and 2 100. Clocks or watches were not permitted but the time appeared on the computers at each activity transition, eg, 0900, 0930, 1000, 1300. During the periods when subjects had access to the social areas, a timesampled record was kept oftheir activity. Once every 2 mm the activity of each subject was recorded as lying, sitting, standing, walking,

exercising,

or being

in the

collected to assess the possibility fat and CHO consumption. Food

bathroom.

These

of activity

changes

data

related

were

to

monitoring

access was controlled. After weighing in each morning, a box of food was placed in the food drawer ofeach ofthe three private rooms. This box contained a wide variety of foods including some meal items, and conventional snack-food items including liquid items (see Appendix A for a complete list of foods), that could be consumed at any time during the experimental day (from 0900 to 0900 the next morning). Snack-item portion sizes were designed to contain a roughly equivalent energy content. Subjects were free to request additional units of any items ad libitum. Frozen meal items were available by request throughout the experimental day. To facilitate choice of frozen meal items, subjects were provided with a book containing the pictures from the front of a box of each of the available Food

Downloaded from www.ajcn.org by guest on April 26, 2012

Six (two groups of three) healthy, adult, male, research volunteers ranging in age from 22 to 32 y [26.8 ± 1.3 y (1 ± SE)] participated in l4-d experiments. All subjects were within accepted weight ranges [77.9 ± 5.7 kg, (22)], had low dietary restraint [self-report score of < 10, (23)1, and had no self-reported eating abnormalities [abnormality score of < 15, (24)]. Four of the six subjects reported smoking between 10 and 20 tobacco cigarettes/d (12.5 ± 2.5) and continued to do so during the cxperiment. Subjects received complete medical and psychiatric evaluations, signed consent forms detailing all aspects of the and

AL

ing

Subjects

research,

ET

FAT, TABLE 1 Characteristics

of the three-planned

eating

CARBOHYDRATE, occasions

under

AND

CALORIC

REGULATION

333

each condition Percent weight

Condition

Low fat Medium

Energy

Protein

g

%

g

%

kJ/g

kJ/g

3092 5158 7051

13.9 60.2 107.8

16.9 43.9 57.6

103.4 111.2 111.7

56.0 36.1 26.5

48.1 59.1 61.1

26.0 19.2 14.5

1032 1107 1244

63 58 59

6.09 10.08 12.17

0.63 0.75 1.21

2939

21.0

26.9

107.1

60.9

29.6

16.8

1383

55

4.56

0.14

5076

21.5

16.0

233.7

76.9

30.3

10.0

1472

52

5.06

1.92

6969

19.6

10.6

346.5

83.2

33.5

8.0

1770

47

5.69

1.92

density

of solid items. of fluid items.

in macronutrient

content

macronutrient

to increase

intake

the

opportunity

as a function

of experi-

condition. were

told

that

their

food

intake

was

continuously

monitored by independent observers and were instructed to inform the research monitors via the computerized communication system whenever they ate or drank something, specifying substance and portion. Wrappers for each food were color coded by subject

to facilitate

data

collection.

Trash

was

removed

and

measured daily to validate verbal reports and observer records offood intake and to control for the possibility offood hoarding. Previous studies indicated that these procedures have no significant effect on total daily intake, and subjects under these conditions are sensitive to manipulations affecting daily amount and patterning of food intake (4, 18-20, 21). A snack was defined as the between-meals consumption of any item contained within the box of food not requiring preparation time. The energy derived from coffee or tea in combination with milk and sugar was classified as snack joules. A meal was defined as the consumption ofany ofthe items that required preparation time, including frozen foods and sandwiches, alone or in combination with any ofthe snack food items. For example, cookies consumed individually were a snack item, but when cookies were consumed with any item requiring preparation time, the combination was classified as a meal. Procedures

foods Also,

occasions

and

rated

as acceptable

all ofthe

frozen

and the items contained in the required-eating occasions. subjects received a day of training on the computerized-

performance

before

tasks.

Subjects

reported

to the

laboratory

on

the

the

study,

were

slept

in the laboratory begin at 0900. Subjects pating

in a study

oriented

to living

in the

so that the first experimental were instructed that they

on the effects

facility, day

were

ofcomputer-performance

and

could particistability

over prolonged periods oftime and because nutrition can affect performance they were required to consume all of the food in the required-eating occasions each day. Subjects had access to all other

available

and

allowed

were

ditional

food.

would

vary

foods

during

to supplement

They over

were

the

told

course

the

required-eating

the

eating

that

occasions

occasions

the required-eating

of the

with

ad-

occasions

experiment.

1 provides

a description ofthe macronutrient and energy contents of the required-eating occasions and Appendix B provides a complete description of the foods presented in these required-eating occasions. The required breakfast meals that varied in fat consisted ofeggs, sausages, and a cake item. The required breakfast meals that varied in CHO consisted ofpancakes, syrup, and fruit. The energy content of breakfast was 850 Id (200 kcal) for the low, 1 700 Id (400 kcal) for the medium, and 2450 Id (600 kcal) for the high conditions. The required lunches conTable

sisted

of a sandwich

energy 500,

content

and

700

respectively. consisted afternoon

required

chocolate

snacks

a carbonated was

1300,

for the low,

kcal) The

ofa

and

of lunch

that

medium,

afternoon cupcake

varied

orange 2150,

and

in CHO

beverage.

and

and

snacks milk,

that whereas

consisted

The

1900 Id (300, high conditions,

ofa

varied

in fat

the required savory

snack

mix and carbonated beverage. The energy content of the afternoon snack was 820,J260, and 1650 Id (200, 300, and 400 kcal) for the low, medium, and high conditions, respectively. The three required-eating occasions contained 30#{174} kJ (700 kcal) under the low-calorie condition, 5l00 Id (1200 kcal) under

A list of possible foods was presented to the subjects before the study and they rated the acceptability of each item and whether they had previously consumed that item. All subjects reported having previously eaten the foods contained in the required-eating

day

the medium-calorie

condition,

and

7000

U (1700

kcal)

under the high-calorie condition. The energy differential was accomplished by manipulating the energy derived from either fat or CHO, resulting in six conditions (low CHO, med CHO, high CHO, low fat, med fat, and high fat). The low-CHO and low-fat conditions were similar with respect to fat and CHO contents. In addition, on the first and last day of each study there were no required-eating occasions, ie, subjects self-selected food during the entire day. Table 2 indicates the order of testing

Downloaded from www.ajcn.org by guest on April 26, 2012

frozen items. In addition, subjects had free access to instant coffee, tea, and water at all times. Both meal and snack items were chosen to provide items varying in fat and CHO contents, and to have a similar number of items high in CHO or high in fat. Such conditions provided subjects with a variety of items

Subjects

Fluid densityt

density*

%

density

mental

Solid

fluid

g

Energy

for differential

from

%

t Energy

varying

Weight

g

Highfat

S

Carbohydrate

U

fat

Low carbohydrate Medium carbohydrate High carbohydrate

Fat

FOLTIN

334

ET

AL

TABLE

2 Experimental

design

ondays

1-14

I Group 1 Condition*

Macronutrient Group 2 Condition Macronutrient S

The low-CHO

5158

2

3

4

5

6

None

Medium

Medium

Low

Low

High

High

None

CHO

CHO

Fat

Fat

CHO

CHO

None None

Medium Fat

Medium Fat

High CHO

High CHO

Low Fat

Low Fat

condition

kJ, the high-CHO

contained

condition

6969

2939 kJ, the low-fat condition Id, and the high-fat

condition

analysis

Experimental days began at 0900 and food intake over the next 24 h was analyzed. Intake during the 2 d when there were no required-eating occasions served as the control condition. The data used in the analyses were based on the subjects’ reports offood intake (verified by weighing oftrash). Data were analyzed by using three series ofplanned comparisons involving repeated measures. In the first series ofcomparisons, data obtained under the low-CHO and low-fat conditions were compared with each of the other conditions. In the second set of comparisons, data obtained under the control condition were compared with each of the other conditions. In the third set of comparisons, data obtained under the med-fat, med-CHO, high-fat, and high-CHO conditions were compared. Total energy intake and energy intake of CHO, fat, and protein [estimated as kilocalories from gram intake by using Atwater factors (26)] were analyzed with and without the caloric content of the items in the required-eating occasions. between

Specifically, either

the

the

following

low-CHO

and

comparisons low-fat

and

the

were

made

control

con-

1) total daily intake; 2) intake during the private condition, ie, when the planned occasions occurred, and social condition; 3) intake derived from snacks and meals; and 4) the interaction between condition and private and social-access period intake. The percent of energy intake derived from each ofthe three macronutrients (with and without the required-eating occasions) were analyzed by using comparisons based only on total daily intake. The proportion of time that subjects spent in the resting categories (ie, lying, sitting, standing, and being in the bathroom) under each condition was ditions

with

each

other

8

9

Medium

Medium

Fat

Fat

Medium CHO

Id. None

indicates

12

13

14

Low

Low

High

High

None

CHO

Fat

Fat

None

High Fat

High Fat

Low CHO

Low CHO

None None

condition

control

11

CHO

Medium CHO

3092 Id, the medium-CHO 7051

10

5076 kJ, the medium-fat

condition

condition.

compared with the low-CHO and low-fat conditions and the control condition. Body weight before the first residential day was compared with body weight on the morning after the last day by using a paired t test. The results for each of the four questions answered during the debriefing taste session for each of the eight food groups were compared by using separate repeated-measures analyses of variance (27). Results were considered statistically significant if P < 0.05.

Results Energy

intake

Table 3 summarizes the total energy content of the plannedoccasions and relates this to the energy intake under the

food

control

conditions.

The

tamed,

respectively,

23%,

under

the control

intake,

including

low,

medium,

39%,

and

and 53%

high

ofthe

conditions energy

con-

consumed

condition. Also listed is the total daily energy the planned occasions compared with intake under the control condition. Figure 1 presents daily energy intake with and without the energy contained in the required-eating occasions as a function ofcondition. Under the control condition subjects consumed 13 297 kJ (3200 kcal)/d. When the energy content of the required-eating occasions was included in the data analysis, only under the low-CHO condition was intake significantly less than under the control condition (F1151 = 9.63, P < 0.027). When compared with energy intake under the low-

TABLE 3 Energy content

of the planned

occasions

and energy intake under

each

condition

Condition

Energy

Percent of control

Total energy intake*

Percent of control

U

%

Ici

%

0 2939

0 22

13297± 1356 11297±3314

100 85t

5076

38

13640±3916

103

6969

52 23 39 53

14665±2686 12401 ±4243 14 364 ± 2761 12326±2548

110 93 108 93

condition:

Control LowCHO

MediumCHO HighCHO Lowfat Medium fat Highfat *

I

3092 5158

7051

± SE.

t Significantly

different

from the control

condition,

P


1

0) C) C LU

intake

3 presents

condition (top),

with

quired-food condition,

derived

6276

Id

similar

intake

during

the

pattern

social-access significantly dition

P