(Canis dingo) and the Domestic Dog (Canis familiaris)

4 downloads 0 Views 470KB Size Report
M.T.E received .... 1995; Corbett, 2001; Mech & Boitani, 2003; Miklósi, 2007; Thompson, 1992; ..... 2015; Thomson, 1992; red fox Vulpes vulpes: Henry, 1996).
1

Pattern of Nipple Use by Puppies: A Comparison of the Dingo (Canis

2

dingo) and the Domestic Dog (Canis familiaris)

3

Robyn Hudson1, Heiko G. Rödel2, Marise Trejo Elizalde1, Lourdes Arteaga3, Gerard

4

Kennedy4, Bradley Smith5

5 6

1

7

Distrito Federal

8

2

9

13, Sorbonne Paris Cité, France

Instituto de Investigaciones Biomédicas, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México,

Laboratoire d’Ethologie Expérimentale et Comparée E.A. 4443 (LEEC), Université Paris

10

3

11

Mexico

12

4

13

Australia

14

5

Centro Tlaxcala de Biología de la Conducta, Universidad Autónoma de Tlaxcala, Tlaxcala,

School of Psychology, Counselling and Psychotherapy. Cairnmillar Institute, Victoria,

Central Queensland University, School of Human Health and Social Sciences, Australia

15 16

We thank Lyn Watson and Lyn Whitworth from the Dingo Discovery and Research

17

Sanctuary, and Carolina Rojas for technical and bibliographical assistance. Financial support was

18

provided by Central Queensland University, and the Australian Dingo Foundation. M.T.E received

19

a post-graduate fellowship (294591) from the Mexican national funding agency CONACYT. A

20

travel grant was provided to R.H. by the Université Paris 13, Sorbonne Paris Cité, France.

21 22

Correspondence:

23

Robyn Hudson, Instituto de Investigaciones Biomédicas, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de

24

México, A.P. 70228, Ciudad Universitaria C.P. 04510, Distrito Federal, Mexico

25

Ph. +52 55 5622 3828; Fax. +52 55 50044

26

Email: [email protected] 1

27 28

Bradley Smith, Central Queensland University, School of Human Health and Social Sciences,

29

North Bruce Highway, Rockhampton, Queensland, Australia, 4702. Ph. + 61 7 4923 2813;

30

Fax. +61 7 4930 6460.

31

Email: [email protected]

32 33

Email addresses of further authors:

34

Heiko G. Rödel: [email protected]

35

Marise Trejo Elizalde: [email protected]

36

Lourdes Arteaga: [email protected]

37

Gerard Kennedy: [email protected]

38 39 40 41

2

42

Abstract

43

Surprisingly little information is available about the behaviour of newborn mammals in the

44

functionally vital context of suckling. We have previously reported notable differences in the

45

pattern of nipple use by kittens of the domestic cat and puppies of the domestic dog. Whereas

46

kittens rapidly develop a “teat order”, with each individual using principally one or two

47

particular nipples, puppies show no such pattern. We asked whether the more “chaotic”

48

behaviour seen in puppies of the domestic dog (Canis familiaris) could be the result of

49

relaxed selection due to domestication. In a first test of this hypothesis we studied suckling

50

behaviour in four litters of wild-type captive dingoes (Canis dingo), a canid species that has

51

inhabited the Australian mainland in substantial numbers for at least 5,000 years with

52

minimal human influence. On all measures of individual puppies’ behaviour – time spent

53

attached to nipples, lack of individual use of particular nipples and consequent absence of a

54

“teat order”, lack of synchronized suckling with other littermates, lack of agonistic behaviour

55

– we found no differences between the two species. In conclusion, we suggest that the

56

difference between the pattern of suckling behaviour of kittens of the domestic cat (and other

57

felids) and the domestic dog is not an artifact of domestication, but rather reflects

58

phylogenetic differences between felids and canids as a consequence of their different

59

lifestyles. These findings emphasize the need for comparative studies to avoid simplistic

60

generalizations from one or two species across broad taxonomic groups.

61 62

Key words: Suckling behaviour, “teat order”, sibling competition, Canis dingo, Canis

63

familiaris

64

3

65

All mammals depend for their early growth and survival on the mother’s milk and

66

obtaining this in sufficient quantities represents one of life’s earliest challenges. In particular,

67

most altricial young, blind at birth and often with limited motor abilities, must locate, attach

68

to and suck nipples without direct maternal assistance. In addition, as milk is energetically

69

costly for mothers to produce (Loudon & Racey, 1987; Speakman, 2008), it usually

70

represents a limiting resource for which littermates must compete (Drummond, Vázquez,

71

Sánchez-Cólon, Martínez-Gómez, & Hudson, 2000; Drummond, 2006). Despite the

72

importance of suckling, little information is available and then only for a few species on the

73

behaviour of altricial young in this vital context (Drummond, 2006; Hudson & Trillmich,

74

2008; Mock & Parker, 1997). Studying this behaviour, even in captivity, is notoriously

75

difficult; altricial young are usually hidden in dens, nests or pouches, human interference may

76

lead to them being abandoned, and they are often vigorously defended by well-armed

77

mothers or other caretakers.

78

For some years we have been studying mother-offspring interactions in the domestic

79

cat (Felis silvestris catus) and domestic dog (Canis familiaris). Consistent with other reports

80

in the literature (domestic cat: Ewer, 1959; Rosenblatt, 1971; puma Puma concolor: Pfeifer,

81

1980; snow leopard Panthera uncia: McVittie, 1978; Eurasian lynx Lynx lynx: Glukhova &

82

Naidenko, 2014), we found that newborn kittens rapidly develop a clear order in the

83

individual use of particular nipples, which they vigorously defend against encroaching

84

littermates (Hudson, Raihani, González, Bautista, & Distel, 2009; Hudson & Distel, 2013;

85

Hudson, 2014; Raihani, González, Arteaga, & Hudson, 2009). In contrast, and also in accord

86

with a previous report (Rheingold, 1963), we found that puppies of the domestic dog do not

87

do this, and that their pattern of suckling differs in several respects to the cat. Puppies of

88

various breeds and under various conditions of maintenance, showed no clear order in

89

individual nipple use, they showed no agonistic behaviour during suckling, and in contrast to 4

90

kittens which usually all attach to nipples in coordinated suckling bouts, puppies often

91

attached in a seemingly haphazard manner, with some suckling while others continued

92

searching for nipples, moved around the nest area, slept or played (Arteaga, Rödel, Trejo

93

Elizalde, González, & Hudson, 2013).

94

According to the literature, free ranging domestic dogs differ notably from wild

95

canids in relation to the quality and frequency of reproductive activity (seasonality, age of

96

first reproduction, frequency of oestrus, pair bonding) and parental feeding behaviours

97

(nursing, provisioning the mother and young by the male or other pack members) (Boitani,

98

Ciucci, & Andreoli, 1995; review by Lord, Feinstein, Smith, & Coppinger, 2013). The

99

authors argue that these differences reflect the domestic dog’s adaptation to the human niche

100

and access to readily available resources, which has subsequently relaxed the selection

101

pressure for extended and bi-parental care. We speculated (Arteaga et al., 2013) that the

102

seemingly less coordinated suckling behaviour in the dog might have been the result of

103

relaxed selection during this species’ much longer and closer association with humans than

104

has been the case for the domestic cat. Whereas archaeological evidence suggests that the

105

presumed wolf (Canis lupus) ancestor of the domestic dog started its association with human

106

hunter-gatherer societies 10,000 or more years ago (Clutton-Brock, 1995; Miklósi, 2007;

107

Shannon et al., 2015; Wang et al., 2015), the presumed ancestor of the domestic cat (Felis

108

lybica) is thought to have done so about 4,000 years ago with the advent of agriculture and

109

the proliferation of sedentary human communities (Serpell, 2014). Further evidence for the

110

longer history of domestication of the dog than of the cat is the fact that dogs are no longer

111

able to form reproductively self-sustaining populations independent of human communities

112

(Boitani et al., 1995), whereas domestic cats readily do so, sometimes to the point of posing a

113

threat to native wildlife, particularly of island ecologies (Fitzgerald & Turner, 2000). In order

114

to decide whether observed differences in the pattern of suckling behaviour between kittens 5

115

and puppies are due to different degrees of domestication, or rather represent phylogenetic

116

differences in the organization of this vital aspect of mother-young behaviour, it would be

117

necessary to investigate the pattern of suckling in non-domesticated, taxonomically close

118

relatives of the dog.

119

In a first step, it was our aim in the present study to investigate suckling behaviour in

120

puppies of the Australian wild dog or dingo Canis dingo (International Commission on

121

Zoological Nomenclature, 1957; Crowther, Fillios, Colman, & Letnic, 2014) during the early

122

den period, and to compare this with our previous findings in the domestic dog.

123

Although the precise origin of the dingo is contentious, it is likely descended from an

124

ancient population of a south-east Asian canid, and arrived on the Australian continent

125

around 5,000+ years ago (Smith & Savolainen, 2015). Since this time, the dingo has lived

126

independent of human influence, and from other dog or wolf populations, making it unique

127

among the world’s canids. The dingo inhabits most of the Australian mainland in appreciable

128

numbers, and as a top order predator has a profound influence on the structure and function

129

of Australian ecosystems (Glen, Dickman, Soulé, & Mackey, 2007; Newsome, Ballard,

130

Crowther, Dellinger, Fleming et al., 2015). Although the social organisation of dingoes is

131

highly variable (according to habitat, prey, climate and human activity), in general, they are a

132

highly social species that live in a family or social group, maintain a distinct territory, and

133

hunt cooperatively (Smith, 2015). They are seasonal breeders, giving birth to one litter of

134

typically 3 to 6 puppies a year during the winter months (May–August). The puppies are born

135

into a den where the mother remains for extended periods during the first postnatal weeks,

136

and where she is provisioned by her mate (Lord et al., 2013; Smith, 2015). Thus, and

137

importantly for the purpose of our study, the dingo’s pattern of reproduction and parental care

138

is more similar to that of its presumed ancestor the wolf, than is that of the domestic dog

6

139

(Boitani et al., 1995; Corbett, 2001; Mech & Boitani, 2003; Miklósi, 2007; Thompson, 1992;

140

Thompson, Rose, & Kok, 1992).

7

141 142 143

Methods

Animals, Housing Conditions and Sample Sizes

144

Dingo: Twelve dingo puppies from four litters (born in May of 2013) participated in

145

this study (Table 1). All parents were captive born and raised at the Dingo Discovery Centre

146

and Research Sanctuary (DDC), a privately run facility located in Victoria, Australia, and

147

passed a genetic analysis of purity conducted by the University of New South Wales,

148

Australia (Wilton, 2001). Breeding pairs were housed in enclosures (9 m x 1.5 m or 13.5 m2)

149

comprising 7 m of flooring made of quarry rubble (gravel) and a 2 m section of concrete

150

where a wooden whelping box (940 mm L x 635 mm W x 860 mm H) was positioned.

151

Shredded paper was placed inside the whelping box and was replaced daily or as required. A

152

corrugated iron roof covered 2 m of the enclosure above the concrete section, and the

153

remaining roof was covered with wire mesh to prevent escape. Enclosure walls were 2 m

154

high. All enclosure walls were constructed with wire mesh, with the addition of 1.2 m high

155

sheets of corrugated iron between enclosures to act as a windbreak and to prevent fighting

156

between neighbours. Access to one of seven large grassed exercise yards that included natural

157

dens, large hollow logs, trees, rocky areas and enrichment objects was provided daily. After

158

whelping, access to these yards was limited, and occurred on an ad-hoc basis.

159 160

--- Insert Table 1 about here ---

161 162

The mothers were accustomed to human handling and presence, and appeared well

163

adjusted to the study conditions. Socialization of the puppies began at 1–3 days of age, with

164

puppies handled gently for brief periods by two of the staff members (including weighing),

165

until they were about 4–5 weeks of age. Adult dingoes were fed a combination of dry dog 8

166

food and fresh chicken (carcass or ground carcass mince). The diet of the mother was

167

changed 14 days prior to the estimated whelping date to a higher energy food, and mothers

168

were fed twice a day (morning and evening). This continued until the puppies started to be

169

weaned to semi-solid food when their teeth had fully erupted at approximately 21 days of age

170

(at the discretion of the sanctuary staff). The food consisted of dry puppy food and chicken

171

mince soaked in warm water. Puppies were fed twice a day (morning and evening) separately

172

from the adults.

173

Dog: Data from 42 puppies from nine mothers, reanalysed from a previous study

174

(details of mothers, puppies and methods in Arteaga et al., 2013) contributed to the present

175

report. Briefly, the animals were pets kept in different private homes, they were of various

176

breeds, ages and parity, and were fed a variety of diets and maintained under a variety of

177

conditions.

178 179 180

Procedures Dingo: Den behaviours were recorded continuously using a small surveillance camera

181

(Swann Pro 530) and a digital video recorder (Swann 4 channel DVR, model DVR4-4000).

182

Video recordings were made at D1 resolution, at a frame rate of 20 fps, and max bit rate 1024

183

kbps. Cameras were installed in the top corner of four dens at least 24 hours prior to birth.

184

Recording began at least 24 hours prior to birth, and ceased when the puppies started

185

spending more time outside the den box (or at 21 days, whatever occurred first). This resulted

186

in continuous post partum footage totalling between 17 and 21 days except for a total of 5

187

days of lost footage due to power outages and destruction of camera equipment by dingoes.

188

Individual puppies could be distinguished by distinctive coat markings. As in our previous

189

report on suckling behaviour in the domestic dog (Arteaga et al., 2013), the main behaviours 9

190

scored were the frequency and time each puppy spent attached to which nipple. Like most

191

dogs, dingoes typically have four pairs of nipples, which we labelled from anterior to

192

posterior as rows 1–4, and whether the nipples were on the left or on the right side of the

193

mother’s ventrum. We also noted any incidents of apparent contest behaviour such as

194

unattached puppies pushing with their head and muzzle against the muzzle of attached

195

littermates apparently trying to dislodge them (cf. Hudson et al., 2009; Hudson & Distel,

196

2013 for kittens). However, as no such behaviours were recorded (see also results of the

197

inter-observer reliability check below) this is not further considered.

198

Dog: A detailed report of procedures is given in Arteaga et al. (2013). Briefly, so as

199

not to disturb mothers and at the request of some owners, we did not attempt to alter or

200

standardize rearing conditions in any way. Soon after birth, puppies were fitted with a

201

coloured neck ribbon for individual identification, and where possible litters were filmed

202

every second or third day during a single nursing session. For this, mothers were separated

203

from their litters an hour before recording to increase their motivation to nurse and of the

204

puppies to suckle. This was generally effective, and nursing was then filmed for 30 min or

205

until the mother left the litter for more than 3 min, which ever was first. This resulted in the

206

recording of an average of 8.1 sessions per litter. The behaviours scored were the same as

207

those described for the dingo above.

208 209 210

Ethics Note Dingo mothers gave birth with no natal deaths, raised their young to weaning without

211

apparent difficulty, and allowed observation and manipulation of their puppies by sanctuary

212

staff without protest. Puppies were weighed approximately every third day from birth to

213

check weight gain as an indicator of general health. All puppies survived to the end of the 10

214

study (with the exception of the two smallest puppies in Opal’s litter which were euthanized

215

by the owner on postnatal day 1, and a puppy in Freckle’s litter which disappeared from

216

unknown causes on postnatal day 3 but was possibly eaten by the mother), and were sold as

217

pets or to zoos or fauna parks. All methods were approved by the Central Queensland

218

University Animal Ethics Committee, project number A12/11-293. For the dogs, details of

219

treatment of the animals are given in Arteaga et al. (2013). Use of them for this study

220

complied with the guidelines for the care and use of animals in research of the American

221

Psychological Association, and according to the National Guide for the Production, Care and

222

Use of Laboratory Animals, Mexico (Norma Oficial Mexicana NOM-062-200-1999).

223 224

Data Analysis and Sample Sizes

225

Statistical analyses were done with the programme R, version 3.1.1 (R Core Team,

226

2014). The sample sizes available for analysis were 12 dingo puppies from the four litters that

227

could be reliably distinguished by distinctive fur markings, and 42 puppies of the domestic

228

dog from nine litters for which we had sufficient data for comparison with the dingo sample.

229

To enable a direct comparison of findings for the two species we reanalysed the data from

230

Arteaga et al. (2013) so as to correspond to our analysis here for the dingo, and we have

231

modified the previous graphical representations accordingly. However, as reported in Results

232

and Discussion, this did not change the findings or conclusions of our previous study in the

233

dog.

234

First, we aimed to compare different parameters observed during approximately

235

equivalent 30-minute observations between puppies of the dingo and the dog. For this, we

236

calculated average values of the total time spent attached to nipples and the time spent

237

attached to a particular nipple, averaged for each individual puppy over all available nursing 11

238

sessions (32 sessions, always 8 per litter in the dingo; 68 sessions, on average 7.6 in the dog)

239

during the first three postnatal weeks. Comparisons were done with linear mixed-effects

240

models (LMM), based on restricted maximum likelihood estimates using the package lme4

241

(Bates, Maechler, & Bolker, 2014). The identity of the litter was used as a random factor.

242

We then analysed whether and how the cumulative number of nipples used by

243

individual puppies (dependent variable) increased over the first four consecutive nursing

244

sessions (factor with 4 levels) observed during the first two postnatal weeks when suckling

245

was most frequent and sessions that were comparable with those of the previous dog study

246

could be most readily obtained. Generalised linear mixed effects models (GLMM) for

247

Poisson distributed were used, including litter identity and puppy identity as random factors.

248

Furthermore, we included individual-level random effects to account for potential effects of

249

overdispersion (Browne, Subramanian, Jones, & Goldstein, 2005).

250

Third, we explored the distribution of the time and the frequency per nursing session

251

that individual puppies of the two species were attached to mothers’ different nipples. For

252

this, these two variables were plotted versus the ranked attachment time to nipples as well as

253

their frequency of use. Purported preferences in using particular nipples would be evident

254

from a sharp decrease in the duration or in the frequency of attachment to nipples other than

255

to the most used nipple (rank 1, see Fig. 2). Furthermore, we checked whether the (negative)

256

slope of this association differed between puppies of the dingo and dog by testing the

257

interaction between the ranked time or frequency of use (covariates) and species (factor with

258

2 levels). Linear mixed-effects models were used, including litter and puppy identity as

259

random factors.

260

Finally, we tested for correlations across time in puppies’ proportional duration and

261

frequency of nipple use between two time intervals, the first postnatal week and the

262

second/third postnatal weeks. Again, LMM models were used, including litter and puppy 12

263

identity as random factors. For both time intervals and in the dingo as well as in the dog, the

264

duration and the frequency of attachment to mothers’ nipples were adjusted to a normal

265

distribution by log[x+1] transformation. For all associations and comparisons tested, we

266

calculated Nagelkerke’s Pseudo-R2, which can be interpreted as a measure of the proportion

267

of variance explained (Magee, 1990; Nagelkerke, 1991).

268

We checked all models for homogeneity of variances and goodness of fit by plotting

269

residuals versus fitted values. In addition, normality of the model residuals of linear mixed-

270

effects models was assured by checking normal probability plots (Faraway, 2006). P-values

271

were calculated by type III Wald Chi square tests.

272

In an inter-observer reliability check a second observer, experienced in the evaluation

273

of suckling behaviour in kittens and domestic dog puppies but blind to the results obtained

274

for the dingo puppies, re-evaluated a sample of the video material. Because we were

275

interested in possible agonistic behaviour among littermates during suckling as well as in

276

nipple use, we chose to re-evaluate the behaviour in one of the larger litters (mother Petal)

277

where all puppies survived. The behaviour of four puppies (33% of total number of subjects),

278

during 8 sessions (25% of total number of sessions) was re-analyzed. Because of lack of

279

familiarity with the dingo puppies and caution in reliably distinguishing among such

280

similarly-coloured individuals, the second observer only felt confident in registering details

281

for approximately half as many nipple attachments as the first, highly experienced observer.

282

Nevertheless, good correspondence was found between these observations and the

283

corresponding records for the first observer. Thus, neither observer registered any agonistic

284

interactions although explicitly instructed to look for these. The two observers were also in

285

100% agreement on the number of different nipples (4 to 5) each of the puppies suckled. For

286

each nipple attachment they showed 88% agreement on which puppy was attached to which

287

nipple. In all cases in which the two observers differed, the puppy was recorded as being on a 13

288

neighboring nipple, either beside or opposite. Furthermore, an intra-class correlation based on

289

1,000 permutations (library rptR, Schielzeth & Nakagawa, 2013) revealed a high

290

concordance in the attachment times quantified by the 2 observers for each of these events

291

(R2 = 0.939, CI95%[0.806, 0.980], N = 17, P < 0.001).

292

14

293 294 295 296

Results

Comparison of Suckling Behaviour in Dingo and Dog Puppies The total time that individual puppies were attached to nipples during a nursing

297

session, the average number of nipples they used per session as well as the mean time they

298

were attached to a particular nipple did not differ significantly between puppies of the dingo

299

and the domestic dog (Table 2).

300 301

--- Insert Table 2 about here ---

302 303

Moreover, in a high percentage of nursing sessions in both dingoes (15 out of 32;

304

46.9%) and dogs (30 out of 68; 44.1%), not all puppies were attached to nipples but were

305

either apparently searching for a nipple on the mother’s ventrum, moving around the den or

306

nest area, playing with littermates or apparently sleeping. And finally, in dingoes as in dogs,

307

during suckling we did not observe any directly agonistic behaviour or behaviour that could

308

be interpreted as competition for particular nipples.

309 310 311

Cumulative Use of Different Nipples In the dingo (GLMM:  32 = 12.10, PseudoR2 = 0.249, P = 0.007) as well as in the

312

domestic dog (  32 = 59.85, PseudoR2 = 0.342, P < 0.001), the cumulative number of nipples

313

used by individual puppies increased significantly across the first four nursing sessions

314

recorded during the first and second postnatal week (post hoc comparisons in Fig. 1). By the

315

end of the sampling period all puppies had used several nipples, and some individuals had

316

used all. The slope of this increase did not differ between puppies of the dingo and the dog, as

15

317

indicated by the non-significant interaction of nursing session × species (  32 = 1.30, PseudoR2 =

318

0.006, P = 0.73).

319 320

--- Insert Fig. 1 about here ---

321 322

Distribution of Individual Nipple Use

323

The percentage of the total time that puppies of the two species spent attached to

324

different nipples as well as their proportional frequency of the use of different nipples,

325

showed right-skewed distributions in ranking, evenly decreasing in all cases (Fig. 2a–c). The

326

negative slopes of these right-skewed distributions did not differ significantly between

327

puppies of the dingo and dog with respect to proportional time that individual puppies were

328

attached to the different nipples (LMM: % time × species: 12 = 0.46, PseudoR2 = 0.003, P =

329

0.50) or to the proportional frequency of nipple use (% frequency × species: 12 = 0.24,

330

PseudoR

331

puppies show greater use of one or two particular nipples, as would be indicated by a sharp

332

right-skewed drop in the staircase function. Indirectly, this also indicates that the puppies

333

showed no particular preference in the use of nipples with respect to location on the female’s

334

ventrum.

2

= 0.001, P = 0.62). Together, these data indicate that in neither species did the

335 336

--- Fig. 2 about here ---

337 338 339

Lack of Correlations Across Time in Nipples Use There was no indication of a significant association across time in nipple use, i.e.

340

between the first postnatal week and the second and third postnatal week, with respect to the

341

percentage of the total time that the puppies of the two species spent attached to different 16

342

nipples (LMM: dingo: 12 = 0.45, PseudoR2 = 0.005, P = 0.50; dog: 12 = 0.41, PseudoR2 = 0.001,

343

P = 0.52), or to their proportional frequency in the use of different nipples (dingo: 12 = 1.29,

344

PseudoR

2

= 0.014, P = 0.26; dog: 12 = 1.94, PseudoR2 = 0.006, P = 0.16).

345

17

346 347 348

Discussion

For all of the behavioural measures considered in the present study the pattern of

349

nipple use by dingo puppies did not differ from the pattern we had previously found in

350

puppies of the domestic dog (Arteaga et al., 2013). During nursing sessions, dingo and dog

351

puppies spent similar amounts of time attached to nipples, they attached to a similar number

352

of different nipples, and they spent similar amounts of time attached to any one nipple before

353

releasing it. As in the dog, dingo puppies did not show a clear order in the use of particular

354

nipples but rather across nursing sessions each puppy used several different nipples.

355

Moreover, in both dingoes and dogs, in close to half of observed nursing session one or more

356

puppies did not attach to nipples, nor appeared motivated to do so as indicated by them

357

moving around the den, sleeping or playing. This is in contrast to the cat in which in our

358

experience the whole litter usually attaches to nipples during a nursing session, and

359

particularly during the first two postnatal weeks (Hudson et al., 2009; Raihani et al., 2009; R.

360

Hudson, own observations). In both dingo and dog puppies, overtly agonistic behaviour

361

during suckling or behaviour that could be interpreted as competition for nipples was never

362

seen. This contrasts with reports of agonistic behaviour among suckling kittens and piglets

363

(Hudson et al., 2009; review in Hudson & Distel, 2013) but is consistent with the apparent

364

lack of such behaviour among wolf pups (Packard, Mech, & Ream, 1992).

365

As for many other canid and felid species, the behaviour of dingoes is extremely

366

difficult to study in the wild. They are highly mobile, occupy large territories, they are wary

367

and difficult to observe in most environments, and the identification of individuals is often

368

difficult. Regarding captive studies, only few sanctuaries in Australia (or the world) breed

369

dingoes, limiting the possibility to obtain large samples (Smith & Watson, 2015). The DDC

370

where the present study was conducted is the largest breeding sanctuary presently in 18

371

existence, but because dingoes (like wolves but in contrast to domestic dogs) only bred once

372

a year, even here obtaining a closely monitored large sample would require a study of several

373

years. Nevertheless, returning to the question originally motivating this study, we may

374

tentatively conclude that the pattern of nipple use by the domestic dog, so similar to the dingo

375

and so different to the domestic cat and other felids, is unlikely to be an artefact of

376

domestication but rather represents a phylogenetic difference. Despite our small sample size,

377

the present findings for the dingo seem to be reliable. The parents were all pure dingoes, that

378

is, genetic analysis did not show any domestic dog alleles in their DNA (Wilton, 2001). They

379

were habituated to human presence and permitted handling of the puppies by familiar

380

caretakers without protest. Nevertheless, the effect of captive conditions such as the provision

381

of food and the presence of neighbouring breeding pairs is not known, and despite the

382

difficulties mentioned above, ultimately the validity of the findings needs to be checked by

383

studies of wild dingoes and their litters in natural dens.

384

However, in support of the validity of the main finding of similarity in the suckling

385

behaviour of puppies of the dingo and domestic dog, and differences compared to the

386

behaviour of kittens of the domestic cat, we note the consistency in reports on cats and other

387

felids tested under various conditions of captivity although also often based on only few

388

litters. For example, highly similar patterns of sucking behaviour in kittens have been

389

reported in studies of animals reared in confined laboratory cages (Rosenblatt, 1971) and in

390

unconfined house cats (Ewer, 1959; Hudson et al., 2009; Raihani et al., 2009), as well as in

391

other felid species kept under a range of captive conditions (Glukhova & Naidenko, 2014;

392

McVittie, 1978; Pfeifer, 1980). Such similarity among felid species, but so different to the

393

domestic dog and now apparently to the dingo, suggest that in general the young of these two

394

taxonomic groups, canids and felids, might indeed have evolved different patterns of

395

behaviour in the nursing context. Although this clearly needs to be confirmed by the 19

396

investigation of additional species from both taxonomic groups, and where possible in the

397

wild, we may already ask how such differences might be explained?

398

As we suggested previously, one possibility might relate to the generally different

399

lifestyles of these two taxonomic groups (Arteaga et al., 2013). As obligate carnivores,

400

usually lone hunters (Bradshaw, 2006; Fitzgerald & Turner, 2000; Sunquist & Sunquist,

401

2002), and dependent on their agility and climbing ability to escape danger, it should be

402

advantageous for feline mothers to maintain a minimum number of active, burdensome

403

mammary glands. This implies, however, that the kittens or cubs should ensure an adequate

404

milk supply by regularly sucking their “own” nipple, thereby preventing its involution (Ewer,

405

1959; R. Hudson, own observations, Kim, Easter & Hurley, 2001 in pigs Sus scrofa; see

406

Hudson & Distel, 2013; Hudson, 2014 for reviews of the possible functional significance of

407

the teat order in kittens and piglets). By contrast, more omnivorous and group- or pair-living

408

canid mothers may have been under less selection pressure to minimize the number of active

409

mammary glands. This may be particularly the case as mothers are able to remain in the den

410

with their litter for long periods due to provisioning with food by their mate or the pack

411

(wolf: Mech, Wolf, & Packard, 1999; Mech & Boitani, 2003; dingo: Corbett, 2001; Smith,

412

2015; Thomson, 1992; red fox Vulpes vulpes: Henry, 1996). Such provisioning, and with

413

apparently similar consequences for nursing/suckling behaviour, is not confined to canids. A

414

similar case is also provided by a rodent, the naked mole-rat Heterocephalus glaber. In this

415

species the breeding female of a colony is provisioned by other colony members, and the

416

young have continuous, non-synchronized and apparently non-competitive access to nipples

417

(Sherman et al., 1999). The young of canid and other mammalian mothers who receive social

418

support (with confirmation from further species certainly needed) are able to sample a range

419

of nipples at any moment and with little or no obstruction from littermates, who at any

420

particular time may not be similarly motivated to suckle (cf. Rheingold, 1963). This would 20

421

imply, however, that the young can extract milk from the often considerably enlarged

422

mammary glands at will. Although milk let-down in the dog, as in other mammals, is

423

stimulated by the release of the hormone oxytocin from the posterior pituitary in response to

424

sucking (Pickford, 1960), to our knowledge nothing is known about the operation of this

425

mechanism in response to the repeated, intermittent and apparently uncoordinated suckling

426

by individual puppies. By contrast, a litter of kittens obtains milk in a single let-down of only

427

a few seconds per nursing episode after several minutes of combined sucking (Hudson et al.,

428

2009), a situation possibly serving rapid milk transfer to the young by a mother who must

429

then soon leave them to resume hunting.

430

Given increasing interest in the ontogeny of individual differences in behavioural

431

phenotypes (Stamps & Groothuis, 2010; Trillmich & Hudson, 2011), including in the

432

potential role of littermates in shaping these (Drummond et al., 2000; Hudson & Trillmich,

433

2008; Hudson, Bautista, Reyes-Meza, Morales Montor, & Rödel, 2011; Bautista, Zepeda,

434

Reyes-Meza, Martínez-Gómez, Rödel & Hudson, 2015), it is important to have an

435

appreciation of the range of behavioural patterns and adaptive specializations in such a

436

functionally vital and ubiquitous context for mammals as nursing by mothers and suckling by

437

the young. There is a clear need for more information on the diversity of behavioural patterns

438

and their physiological underpinnings here.

439 440

21

441

References

442 443

Arteaga, L., Rödel, H. G., Trejo Elizalde, M., González, D. & Hudson, R. (2013). The pattern

444

of nipple use before weaning among littermates of the domestic dog. Ethology, 119,

445

12–19.

446

Bates, D., Maechler, M. & Bolker, B. (2014). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen

447

and S4. http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4: R package version 1.1–7.

448

Bautista, A., Zepeda, J. A., Reyes-Meza, V., Martínez-Gómez, M., Rödel, H. G. & Hudson,

449

R. (2015). Contribution of within-litter interactions to individual differences in early

450

postnatal growth in the domestic rabbit. Animal Behaviour, 108, 145–153.

451

Boitani, L., Ciucci, F. P. & Andreli, G. (1995). Population biology and ecology of feral dogs

452

in central Italy. In J. Serpell (Ed.), The domestic dog. (pp. 217–244). Cambridge, UK:

453

Cambridge University Press.

454 455

Bradshaw, J. W. S. (2006). The evolutionary basis for the feeding behavior of domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) and cats (Felis catus). Journal of Nutrition, 136, 1927S–1931S.

456

Browne, W. J., Subramanian, S. V., Jones, K. & Goldstein, H. (2005). Variance partitioning

457

in multilevel logistic models that exhibit overdispersion. Journal of the Royal

458

Statistical Society: Series A, 168, 599–613.

459 460 461 462 463 464

Clutton-Brock, J. (1995). Origin of the dog: domestication and early history. In J. Serpell (Ed.), The domestic dog. (pp. 7–20). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Corbett, L. K. (2001). Dingoes in Australia and Asia. 2nd edition. Marlston, South Australia: JB Books. Crowther, M., Fillios, M., Colman, N. & Letnic, M. (2014). An updated description of the Australian dingo (Canis dingo Meyer, 1793). Journal of Zoology, 293, 192–203.

22

465 466

Drummond, H. (2006). Dominance in vertebrate broods and litters. Quarterly Review of Biology, 81, 3–32.

467

Drummond, H., Vázquez, E., Sánchez-Cólon, S., Martínez-Gómez, M. & Hudson, R. (2000).

468

Competition for milk in the domestic rabbit: survivors benefit from littermate deaths.

469

Ethology, 106, 511–526.

470

Ewer, R. F. (1959). Suckling behaviour in kittens. Behaviour, 15, 146–162.

471

Faraway, J. J. (2006). Extending the linear model with R. Generalized linear, mixed effects

472

and nonparametric regression models. Boca Raton, USA: Chapman & Hall.

473

Fitzgerald, B. M. & Turner, D. C. (2000). Hunting behaviour of domestic cats and their

474

impact on prey populations. In D. C. Turner & P. Bateson (Eds.), The domestic cat.

475

The biology of its behaviour. 2nd edition. (pp. 151–175). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge

476

University Press.

477

Glen, A., Dickman, C., Soulé, M. & Mackey, B. (2007). Evaluating the role of the dingo as a

478

trophic regulator in Australian ecosystems. Austral Ecology, 32, 492–501.

479

Glukhova, A. & Naidenko, S. (2014). Suckling behavior in Eurasian lynx (Lynx lynx L.)

480

cubs: characteristics and correlation with competitive interactions. Zoo Biology, 33,

481

388–393.

482 483 484 485 486

Henry, J. D. (1996). Red fox. The catlike canine. Washington D.C., USA: Smithsonian Institution Press. Hudson, R. (2014). Behavioural epiphenomena revisited: reply to Skok and Škorjanc. Ethology, 120, 739–741. Hudson, R. & Trillmich, F. (2008). Sibling competition and cooperation in mammals:

487

challenges, developments and prospects. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 62,

488

299–307.

23

489

Hudson, R., Raihani, G., González, D., Bautista, A. & Distel, H. (2009). Nipple preference

490

and contests in suckling kittens of the domestic cat are unrelated to presumed nipple

491

quality. Developmental Psychobiology, 51, 322–332.

492

Hudson, R., Bautista, A., Reyes-Meza, V., Morales Montor, J. & Rödel, H. G. (2011). The

493

effect of siblings on early development: a potential contributor to personality

494

differences in mammals. Developmental Psychobiology, 53, 564–574.

495 496 497

Hudson, R. & Distel, H. (2013). Fighting by kittens and piglets during suckling: what does it mean? Ethology, 119, 353–359. International Commission on Zoological Nomencalture (1957). Opinion 451. Use of the

498

plenary powers to secure that the specific name “dingo” Meyer, 1793, as published in

499

the contribution “Canis dingo”, still be the oldest available name of the dingo of

500

Australia (class Mammalia). In F. Hemming (Ed.), Opinions and declarations

501

rendered by the International Commission on Zoological Nomenclature (pp. 329–

502

338). London, UK: International Trust for Zoological Nomenclature.

503

Kim, S. W., Easter, R. A. & Hurley, W. L. (2001). The regression of unsuckled mammary

504

glands during lactation in sows: The influence of lactation stage, dietary nutrients, and

505

litter size. Journal of Animal Science, 79, 2659–2668.

506

Lord, K., Feinstein, M., Smith, B. & Coppinger, R. (2013) Variation in reproductive traits of

507

members of the genus Canis with special attention to the domestic dog (Canis

508

familiaris). Behavioural Processes, 92, 131–142.

509 510 511 512

Loudon, A. S. & Racey, P. A. (1987). Reproductive energetics in mammals. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press. Magee, L. (1990). R^2 measures based on Wald and likelihood ratio joint significance tests. American Statistician, 44, 250–253.

24

513 514 515 516 517 518 519 520 521 522 523 524 525 526 527 528 529 530 531 532 533

McVittie, R. (1978). Nursing behavior of snow leopard cubs. Applied Animal Ethology, 4, 159–168. Mech, L. D., Wolf, P. C. & Packard, J. M. (1999). Regurgitative food transfer among wild wolves. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 77, 1192–1195. Mech, L. D. & Boitani, L. (2003). Wolves. Behavior, ecology and conservation. Chicago, USA: University of Chicago Press. Miklósi, Á. (2007). Dog behaviour, evolution, and cognition. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Mock, D. W. & Parker, G. A. (1997). The evolution of sibling rivalry. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Nagelkerke, N. J. D. (1991). A note on a general definition of the coefficient of determination. Biometrika, 78, 691–692. Newsome, T., Ballard, G., Crowther, M., Dellinger, J., Fleming, P., et al. (2015) Resolving the value of the dingo in ecological restoration. Restoration Ecology, 25, 201–208. Packard, J. M., Mech, L. D. & Ream, R. R. (1992). Weaning in an arctic wolf pack: behavioral mechanisms. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 70, 1269–1275. Pfeifer, S. (1980). Role of the nursing order in social development of mountain lion kittens. Developmental Psychobiology, 13, 47–53. R Core Team. (2014). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing, www.R-project.org. Raihani, G., González, D., Arteaga, L. & Hudson, R. (2009). Olfactory guidance of nipple

534

attachment and suckling in kittens of the domestic cat: inborn and learned responses.

535

Developmental Psychobiology, 51, 662–671.

536

Rheingold, H. L. (1963). Maternal behavior in the dog. In H. L. Rheingold (Ed.), Maternal

537

behavior in mammals (pp. 169–207). New York, USA: John Wiley & Sons. 25

538

Rosenblatt, J. S. (1971). Suckling and home orientation in the kitten: a comparative

539

developmental study. In E. Tobach, L. Aronson & E. Shaw (Eds.), The biopsychology

540

of development (pp. 345–410). New York, USA: Academic Press.

541

Schielzeth, H. & Nakagawa, S. (2013). rptR: Reproducibility for Gaussian and non-Gaussian

542

data. R package version 0.6.405/r52. http:/R-Forge.R-project.org/projects/rptr/

543

Serpell, J. A. (2014). Domestication and history of the cat. In D. C. Turner & P. Bateson

544

(Eds.), The domestic cat. The biology of its behaviour. 3rd edition. (pp. 83–100).

545

Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

546

Shannon, L. M., Boyko, R. H., Castelhano, M., Cory, E., Haywood, J. J., McLean, C., …

547

Boyko, A. R. (2015). Genetic structure in village dogs reveals a Central Asian

548

domestication origin. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112, 13639–

549

13644.

550

Sherman, P. W., Braude, S. & Jarves, U. M. (1999). Litter sizes and mammary numbers of

551

naked mole-rats: breaking the one-half rule. Journal of Mammalogy, 80, 720–733.

552

Smith, B. (2015). Biology and behaviour of the dingo. In B. Smith (Ed.), The dingo debate:

553

origins, behaviour and conservation (pp. 25–53). Clayton South, Victoria: CSIRO

554

Publishing.

555

Smith, B. & Savolainen, P. (2015). The origin and ancestry of the dingo. In B. Smith (Ed.),

556

The dingo debate: origins, behaviour and conservation (pp. 55–79). Clayton South,

557

Victoria: CSIRO Publishing.

558

Comment [RH1]: Can someone please check how JCP handles long co-author lists? Various journals do it this way.

Smith, B. & Watson, L. (2015). The role of private sanctuaries in dingo conservation and the

559

management of dingoes in captivity. In B. Smith (Ed.), The dingo debate: origins,

560

behaviour and conservation (pp. 277–299). Clayton South, Victoria: CSIRO

561

Publishing.

26

562

Speakman, J. R. (2008). The physiological costs of reproduction in small mammals.

563

Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological

564

Sciences, 363, 375–398.

565 566 567 568

Stamps, J. & Groothuis, T. G. G. (2010). The development of animal personality: relevance, concepts and perspectves. Biological Reviews, 85, 301–325. Sunquist, M. & Sunquist, F. (2002). Wild cats of the world. Chicago, USA: University of Chicago Press.

569

Thomson, P. C. (1992). The behavioural ecology of dingoes in North-Western Australia: II.

570

Activity patterns, breeding season, and pup rearing. Wildlife Research, 19, 519–530.

571

Thomson, P. C., Rose, K. & Kok, N. E. (1992). The behavioural ecology of dingoes in North-

572

Western Australia: V. Population dynamics and variation in the social system.

573

Wildlife Research, 19, 565–584.

574 575

Trillmich, F. & Hudson, R. (2011). The emergence of personality in animals: the need for a developmental approach. Developmental Psychobiology, 53, 505–509.

576

Wang, G-D., Zhai, W., Yang, H-C., Wang, L., Zhong, L., Liu, Y-H., …. Zhang, Y-P. (2015).

577

Out of southern East Asia: the natural history of domestic dogs across the world. Cell

578

Research, 1–13, www.nature.com/cr

579

Comment [RH2]: As for Shannon et al. above.

Wilton, A. (2001). DNA methods of assessing dingo purity. In C. R. Dickman & D. Lunney

580

(Eds.), A symposium on the dingo (pp. 49–56). Sydney, New South Wales: Royal

581

Zoological Society of New South Wales, Australia.

582

27

583 584 585 586

Table 1

587

Details of the Dingo Mothers and Litters Dingo

Age (yrs)

Parity

Litter size

Sex M

F

Post nataldeaths

Final litter size

Amelia

2

0

5

2

3

0

5

Petal

5

0

6

4

2

0

6

Freckle

8

2

6

2

4

1

5

Opal

7

3

7

3

2

2

5

588 589

28

590 591 592 593

Table 2

594

Comparison of Different Behaviours Observed During Nursing Sessions in Puppies of the

595

Dingo and the Domestic Dog

12

P

602 s ± 53

1.52

0.22

2.1 ± 0.3

2.3 ± 0.2

0.57

0.45

255 s ± 39

286 s ± 33

0.38

0.54

Parameter

Dingo

Domestic dog

Attachment time / session

533 s ± 89

N nipples used /session Attachment time / nipple 596 597

Data are from 12 puppies of the dingo (4 litters) and 42 puppies of the domestic dog (9 litters)

598

recorded during 30-minute observation sessions. Means and 95% confidence intervals are

599

given, and statistical comparisons were done by linear mixed-effects models including litter

600

identity as a random factor. Data for the domestic dog are taken and reanalysed from Arteaga

601

et al., 2013.

602 603

29

604 605

Figure 1. Time course of the cumulative proportion of different nipples used by individual

606

puppies of (a) the dingo and (b) the domestic dog across four recorded nursing sessions

607

during the first 2 postnatal weeks. Solid horizontal lines give medians, boxes indicate 25th

608

and 75th percentiles, and vertical lines show the 10th and 90th percentiles. Sample sizes

609

(number of puppies) are given in the graph. See text for statistics. Data for the domestic dog

610

are taken and reanalysed from Arteaga et al., 2013.

611

30

612 613 614

Figure 2. Distribution of the (a, c) percentage time and (b, d) the percentage frequency that

615

puppies of the dingo (N = 12) and the domestic dog (N = 42) were attached to the different

616

nipples of their mother. Bars indicate means ± 95% confidence intervals, averaged over all

617

puppies. The ranking of the nipples indicates the absolute frequency and time of attachment

618

to the different nipples by each individual puppy, where 1 denotes the nipple per puppy with

619

the longest/most frequent attachment and 8 denotes the nipple with the shortest/least frequent

620

attachment. See text for statistics. Data for the domestic dog are taken and reanalysed from

621

Arteaga et al., 2013.

622 31