Cognitive Function and Cerebral Perfusion During Cocaine Abstinence

6 downloads 134 Views 164KB Size Report
Am J Psychiatry 158:4, April 2001. Clinical Case Conference. Cognitive Function and Cerebral Perfusion. During Cocaine Abstinence. Christopher Gottschalk ...
Clinical Case Conference

Cognitive Function and Cerebral Perfusion During Cocaine Abstinence stration of acute, cocaine-induced cerebral vasospasm in humans (5). Previous investigations have demonstrated John Beauvais, Ph.D. both deficits in neuropsychological performance and abnormalities in brain perfusion or metabolism in chronic Rachel Hart, M.S., L.A.D.C. cocaine abusers and that both of these deficits can improve during abstinence (4, 6, 7). Although prior studies Thomas Kosten, M.D. have indicated that the severity of these flow deficits is related to the degree and duration of drug abuse, their pathophysiology is unknown, and their consequences on brain function are unclear. ecovery from addiction requires motivation to change, understanding of the process of addiction, and One corollary of these findings is that the degree of the ability to form new, stable patterns of behavior. Cognichange in CBF during abstinence correlates with the detive behavior therapy is currently a mainstay of treatment gree of abnormality at the beginning of treatment. Figure for cocaine dependence. Its aim is to change behavior and 1 presents the baseline image of the patient in case 2, Ms. affective responses by teaching coping skills and by adB, in relation to that of a typical comparison subject. dressing and modifying dysfunctional These images have been scaled and colorthought patterns. Through learning and coded to show the same relative activity. practice, a patient can gain increasing con“[H]er passive and Note that in addition to several cortical trol over urges and behaviors that were regions with markedly decreased perfuat times apathetic sion, e.g., orbital and dorsal prefrontal, previously considered to be unmanageable and immutable. In most substance parieto-occipital and frontoparietal, CBF attitude toward abuse treatment programs, counseling in the patient’s cortex is generally lower abstinence were also includes disease-model education, than that of a comparison subject. We anger management, and motivational en- significant barriers have hypothesized that a measure of the hancement in both individual and group change from baseline perfusion will corto her recovery.” settings to provide alternative responses relate with a measure of the capacity to when an addict is faced with unmanagelearn new behavior. able feelings, urges, or circumstances. The cases were chosen to highlight the The capacity to respond to such “psychosocial” inrole of cognitive function and its relation to cerebral pertervention is largely dependent on a patient’s cognitive fusion in recovery from cocaine abuse and to demonstrate flexibility. Intellect, in the conventional sense, varies as a an association between the response to behavioral therfunction of education and socioeconomic background. apy and improvement in cerebral perfusion. Both patients Educational history is relevant in cognitive therapy only as were in drug-free residential care during the two neuroimregards intellect in the intrinsic sense: the ability to learn aging and neuropsychological testing sessions. We suggest and reason and abstract. The case reports presented here that this correlation provides a hypothesis for further clinwere chosen to illustrate the premise that intellect, in this ical study of the relationship between cerebral perfusion less colloquial sense, changes over the period of early reand the response to cognitive behavior therapy in recovery covery from heavy drug exposure. We propose that this from cocaine dependence. change in complex cognitive function is multidetermined, a product of effects of drug abuse at many levels of regulaCase Reports tion of cerebral function, but focus on cerebral perfusion Case 1 in these two cases. Cocaine induces constriction of coronary and cerebral Ms. A, a 34-year-old woman, entered treatment 4 days vessels in both humans and in animal models (1, 2). Over after her last use of cocaine. She had a 19-year history of cocaine abuse: 10 years of intranasal use and 9 years of the last 10 years, several groups of investigators using crack cocaine use. During the 3 months before her adpositron emission tomography and single photon emismission, she reported using varying amounts (0.5–1.5 g) sion computed tomography to measure brain perfusion of cocaine about 3 days per week. Her recent use of alcohave found that chronic cocaine use (usually in the context hol had been limited to about 1 pint of liquor on each of polysubstance abuse) is associated with multiple focal day of cocaine use. She reported two previous substance decreases in cerebral blood flow (CBF) (3, 4). Magnetic resabuse treatments: the first led to no sustained abstinence, and the second she did not complete. onance angiography has provided the first direct demon-

Christopher Gottschalk, M.D.

R

540

Am J Psychiatry 158:4, April 2001

CLINICAL CASE CONFERENCE FIGURE 1. Baseline Perfusion Scans of Ms. B, a Cocaine-Dependent Patient, and One Normal Comparison Subject a Selected Slices

Representative Comparison Subject a

Case 2, Ms. B

Red represents the areas of highest perfusion, and yellow represents areas that are 60%–70% of maximum perfusion. Scans were acquired within 48 hours of last cocaine use for the patient. Both subjects were at rest, with eyes closed, in a dark, quiet room, without active task instructions.

Cognitive behavior therapy. Ms. A placed herself in the early-late stage of recovery on the basis of the Jellinek chart, a self-assessment instrument for staging the progress of addiction (8). From the outset, Ms. A was forthcoming with self-disclosure and receptive of feedback in group therapy, making excellent use of the group in learning how to handle stress. She had no difficulty identifying the consequences of her use, the most obvious external example being her inability to parent her two children. During her second week in treatment, she reported a sporadic craving for cocaine and recognized that these cravings were playing a significant role in her perception of recovery and treatment. This was evidence of a fairly sophisticated ability to draw inferences. However, although she acknowledged the fallacy of her previous perceptions of drug use—“It will make things better”— she was less aware of the degree of denial that occurred in response to her craving for cocaine, manifested in statements such as, “I can control it.” During her third week in treatment, she remained active in group therapy and was often viewed by peers as a leader within the group. This contrasted significantly with her perception of herself as one consumed with insecurity; the feedback she received highlighted one of the greatest barriers to maintaining abstinence: her low self-esteem. She was able to discuss how her chronic feelings of shame and guilt were exacerbated by, but not entirely caused by, her addiction. After reflecting on her relationships, she agreed to a family therapy session and then chose to end the self-described “enabling” relationship she had with her boyfriend. As noted, her cognitive state was impressive at admission and remained so throughout her treatment. She demonstrated the ability to draw inferences, to challenge unproductive thoughts, to recognize cravings and strategize responses to them, and to retain information. There was clearly a commitment on Ms. A’s part to assume responsibility for her recovery by changing thoughts, behaviors, and relationship dynamics. Although some of this profile may reflect familiarity with treatment, she clearly benefited from a high level of motivation combined with the capacity to integrate new Am J Psychiatry 158:4, April 2001

concepts and information and readiness to change. At discharge, Ms. A had been clean and sober for 36 days. She was referred for ongoing treatment and entered a halfway house at discharge.

Neuropsychological testing. This right-handed woman had 13 years of formal education. She performed in the average range on most tests but did show mild impairment in tests that measured simple auditory attention, complex auditory processing, and simple motor repetition. On retesting, her performance fell in the average range on all measures. On her last examination day, Ms. A performed in the average range on most measures but scored above average on one motor test and a complex auditory processing task (the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task) and below average on a verbal learning and memory task (the Auditory Verbal Learning Test). On this last task, mild impairment was noted both in learning and recall after both long and short delays, indicating lack of concentration or effort or both. Overall, Ms. A demonstrated mild gains in performance over a 28-day study period; these were most likely due to the effects of practice. The results of the last day of testing show that although she showed significant improvement on some tasks, her effort was inconsistent during that session.

Perfusion imaging. Statistically significant increases in CBF as determined by the use of statistical parametric mapping from baseline to 2 weeks for Ms. A are shown in Figure 2 (9, 10). The total number of regions that improved was not greater than that obtained by chance (i.e., the set-level inference was not significant); however, some individual brain regions showed significantly increased perfusion in both degree and spatial extent. The largest such cluster included the majority of the right temporal lobe. The other significant areas of improvement included the corresponding contralateral region, the left temporal pole, and the frontal opercular cortex bilaterally. It is interesting to note that Ms. A’s performance on cognitive testing would predict changes in cerebral function in areas related to auditory and language process-

541

CLINICAL CASE CONFERENCE FIGURE 2. Summary Statistical Parametric Map of Increases in Perfusion Over 2 Weeks in Ms. Aa Brain Sections

a

CBF Increases

Location of Slices

The colored areas represent areas of significant change 2 weeks after the baseline study. On the left are orthogonal views of the areas of greatest change in the right hemisphere. On the right are three-dimensional rendered views of the areas of maximum change on the external surface of the brain. The regional changes are less than that expected by chance, relative to the changes found in a comparison group.

ing. The psychometric test results showed some impairment in measured simple auditory attention and complex auditory processing at baseline, which had normalized by the time of the second test session administration and which was also the time of the second scan, analysis of which is presented here. This convergence of outcome data provides support for the correlation of biological and psychological processes.

Case 2 Ms. B, a 32-year-old woman, entered treatment 6 days after her last use of cocaine. She had a 12-year history of cocaine use: 6 years of intranasal cocaine use and 6 years of crack cocaine use. During the 3 months before her admission, she reported using 4–6 g of cocaine per week. Despite a past history of alcohol abuse at ages 18– 20, her use of alcohol in subsequent years was described as infrequent. Although she reported an extensive treatment history, her longest period of abstinence was estimated at “a few months.”

Cognitive behavior therapy. Ms. B also placed herself in the early-late stage on a Jellinek chart of stages of addiction. For Ms. B, participation during group therapy was limited, despite peer and staff encouragement to involve herself. Initially, she recognized only the obvious consequences of her abuse: legal trouble resulting from a possession charge and the subsequent involvement of the Department of Children and Families. Her denial of the scope and severity of her addiction was at least partly related to an apparent inability to draw inferences, a relatively sophisticated cognitive process. Her first treatment plan was designed to address this deficiency. During her second week in treatment, her participation during group therapy improved significantly. She identified with more clarity the progression of her drug use and the parallel progression of its consequences. She described what she would gain—control of her life, re-

542

sumption of parental duties, employment, better health, all of which would improve her overall self-esteem and lose—nothing—if she eliminated drugs from her life. In response to this new recognition of cause and effect relationships, the next period of treatment focused on her underlying perceptions about drug use and the relation of those perceptions to her continued motivation to use. At this point she developed her second treatment plan, focusing on relapse-prevention planning. During her third week in treatment she remained active in group therapy, although she frequently needed prompting to reveal more about herself and her progress in recovery. She identified her tendency to keep problematic issues to herself so as not to risk relapse. Her hesitancy to share information was not due to an inability to understand the nature of therapy for substance dependence. Indeed, she was able to identify relapse warning signs and effective coping strategies for them, but her motivation to address them proactively was inconsistent and, at times, nonexistent. She reported considerable past difficulty in sustaining motivation for recovery, due to, as she saw it, a nonsupportive family. However, she had never utilized self-help techniques or another support system to her advantage. In the hope of reinforcing motivation, therapy again focused on reasons to remain clean. Although she appeared genuinely “tired” of her life as it has been, her passive and at times apathetic attitude toward abstinence were significant barriers to her recovery. She declined an opportunity for family therapy, stating she had “heard it all before.” In order to overcome her passivity, further treatment addressed her patterns of thinking. This revealed that most of her thoughts reflected low self-esteem and low self-expectations. She was able to acknowledge that as long as she remained an active substance abuser, she would be unable to regain control of her life and realize her goals, such as custody of her child. She was encouraged to challenge her automatic, self-defeating thoughts

Am J Psychiatry 158:4, April 2001

CLINICAL CASE CONFERENCE FIGURE 3. Summary Statistical Parametric Map of Increases in Perfusion Over 2 Weeks in Ms. Ba Brain Sections

a

CBF Increases

Location of Slices

The colored areas represent areas of significant change 2 weeks after the baseline study. On the left are orthogonal views of the areas of greatest change in the left hemisphere. On the right are three-dimensional rendered views of the areas of maximum change on the external surface of the brain. The regional changes are more than that expected by chance, relative to the changes found in a comparison group.

and to communicate honestly with peers in treatment and self-help. Utilizing peer relationships in this way may have been complicated by the fact that she was the only woman in her treatment group. However, she was encouraged to make use of her clinician and other clinicians in the program who were female when this appeared to present an obstacle to her. At discharge, Ms. B had been clean and sober for 30 days. As noted, her cognitive state had improved significantly, as evidenced by her ability to draw inferences, challenge unproductive thoughts, identify and strategize for high-risk situations and relapse warning signs and by her ability to retain information. Despite all that she gained cognitively, her ability to assume responsibility for her recovery by proactively changing thoughts, behaviors, and relationship dynamics was questionable. She was referred to another partial day program at discharge.

Neuropsychological testing. This 32-year-old, righthanded woman had 12 years of formal education. In the first test session, she performed in the average range on measures of simple auditory attention and concentration, visuomotor integration, verbal fluency, and learning. On tests that demanded a strong motor effort, her performance was slow (moderate or moderately severe impairment). In addition, her performance in retrieving verbal information after a long delay and on a complex working memory task were significantly below average (mild to moderate impairment). On her second testing session, she maintained her performance on most tests, yet demonstrated improvement on most motor coordination measures and on one task that requires visual scanning (digit vigilance). Her scores on indices of learning and memory were generally lower on repeat testing at 2 weeks than in her first testing session, consistent with the results of other research in cocaine-dependent individuals, suggesting persistence of declarative memory problems over other cognitive deficits (1). At her last

Am J Psychiatry 158:4, April 2001

examination, Ms. B performed in the average range on most measures. Exceptions to this were similar to the ones found in session 2: relative impairments were noted on both delayed memory and on complex mental control tasks. Further gains from the second session were noted on a simple motor repetition (finger oscillation) task. Overall, Ms. B demonstrated mild to moderate gains in performance over the 28-day period, but especially on movement-related tasks. Some of the improvements demonstrated by Ms. B were undoubtedly due to the effects of practice. However, she did not exhibit consistent gains across trials (as one would expect with similar effort); therefore motivation must be considered a mitigating factor. This possibility was more apparent for tasks that demand greater effort.

Perfusion imaging. Increases in CBF from baseline to 2 weeks for Ms. B are shown in Figure 3. The number of regions of change, the set-level inference, is significant at the 5% error level. Several individual regions are also significant in degree of change in perfusion and in spatial extent: left and right occipital poles (including some visual association cortex), and a region of the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. When we compare the results of previous cognitive and motor tests with areas of change in perfusion, we note a correspondence of these regions with the improvement noted in visual scanning tasks and hand-eye coordination. Although the perfusion imaging shows an increase in perfusion in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, the performance on memory tasks was worse on repeat testing; this divergence may reflect a greater demand on a brain region involved in performing working memory tasks. In addition to these specific regional findings, it is the overall (set-level) significance that we emphasize here. This is an index of the proportion of all cor-

543

CLINICAL CASE CONFERENCE

tical tissue with greater perfusion over this test period and provides evidence in this second case that the effect of abstinence on cerebral perfusion was distributed across the entire brain.

Discussion These two patients were matched on many factors that might affect their outcome in treatment (loss of child custody, low self-esteem, and unemployment), yet their overall performance in treatment was notably different. Ms. A was better prepared, psychologically, for taking control of her behavior, and Ms. A benefited to a significant degree from the education offered in treatment. The second patient, Ms. B, began treatment far less prepared to make any meaningful progress in her recovery, despite assessing herself as at the same overall stage in that process as Ms. A. However, Ms. B made significant advances in her level of insight and ability to make inferences about her behavior in relation to her thoughts and reactions. We hypothesize that this difference in cognitive flexibility is, in part, a reflection of the state of cerebral function during the period of treatment and that cerebral perfusion can provide a useful measure of this state. Neuropsychological assessment in both cases was notable for variability among tests, consistent with previous studies in this population (11). Both patients showed below-normal performance on some tests, and most of these test scores improved with time and practice. Although the extent of overall change in perfusion was only significant in Ms. B, in both cases specific brain regions showing greater perfusion were correlated with expected measures of improvement on neuropsychological assessment. Ms. A was able to utilize the tools presented in cognitive behavior therapy to make constructive changes in her self-image and behavior. At discharge, she had reshaped her behavior and thinking patterns in important ways by applying the principles presented in treatment. Direct cognitive testing generally revealed performance in the normal range, with changes detected in a fairly circumscribed area of function. Similarly, testing for changes in perfusion revealed only limited areas of improvement, whereas the overall pattern and degree of change was not significant. From these findings we conclude that only mild deficits in brain function were present in Ms. A at the outset of treatment. In contrast, the second patient, Ms. B, clearly had much greater difficulties with a program of honest selfassessment and open communication, requiring far more effort to recognize the consequences of her addiction. However, in the course of treatment she made significant progress in adopting a new model of the relationship between her mood, thoughts, and behavior. Although direct cognitive testing primarily detected abnormalities in motor function, examination of cerebral perfusion detected significant overall change as well as specific brain regions of change. We propose that this finding of overall significance is an indication of more

544

widespread abnormality in brain function at the outset of treatment. In keeping with this hypothesis, the corresponding progress in Ms. B’s recovery from addiction, as measured by increased readiness for behavioral change, for example, was much greater than that of Ms. A. Future research in this area will determine whether an index of cerebral function, such as the perfusion imaging presented here, can be used to optimize the choice of treatment strategy.

Conclusions Cognitive behavior therapy is currently a mainstay of treatment for cocaine dependence; a complex learning process, its goal is a new relationship among mood, thought, and behavior. The capacity to respond to such “psychosocial” intervention is largely dependent on a patient’s cognitive flexibility. We propose that changes in cognitive function that occur during the period of early recovery from heavy drug abuse are multidetermined, reflecting alterations at many levels of regulation of cerebral function. Previous investigations have demonstrated both deficits in neuropsychological performance and abnormalities in brain perfusion or metabolism in chronic cocaine abusers and that both these deficits can improve during abstinence. We have chosen cerebral perfusion as a marker for recovery from cocaine’s complex pharmacological effects, which clearly outlast its serum half-life. We hypothesized that a measure of the change in cerebral perfusion during early abstinence from cocaine dependence will correlate with a measure of the capacity to learn new behavior. The cases presented here demonstrate an association between the response to cognitive behavior therapy and improvement in cerebral perfusion; this correlation encourages systematic clinical studies of the relationship between cerebral perfusion and the response to cognitive therapy in recovery from cocaine dependence. Received Dec. 19, 1999; revision received Dec. 26, 2000; accepted Dec. 28, 2000. From the Departments of Psychiatry and Neurology, Yale School of Medicine, New Haven, Conn.; and the Substance Abuse Service, VA Connecticut Healthcare System, West Haven. Address reprint requests to Dr. Gottschalk, VACHS—West Haven, Mail Code Psychiatry 116-A, 950 Campbell Ave., West Haven, CT 06516; [email protected] (e-mail). Supported by grants from the National Institute on Drug Abuse (DA-04060, DA-09250, and DA-00454 to Dr. Kosten and DA-00167 to Dr. Gottschalk). The authors thank the staff of the Nuclear Medicine and Substance Abuse Services at the VA Connecticut Healthcare System—West Haven, Conn.; and Cheryl McCullough, Karen Hunkele, Maryjane Caporale, and Suemarie Sanabria for data collection and analysis.

References 1. Vitullo JC, Karam R, Mekhail N, Wicker P, Engelmann GL, Khairallah PA: Cocaine-induced small vessel spasm in isolated rat hearts. Am J Pathol 1989; 135:85–91 2. Flores ED, Lange RA, Cigarroa RG, Hills LD: Effect of cocaine on coronary artery dimensions in atherosclerotic coronary artery disease: enhanced vasoconstriction at sites of significant stenoses. J Am Coll Cardiol 1990; 16:74–79

Am J Psychiatry 158:4, April 2001

CLINICAL CASE CONFERENCE 3. Volkow ND, Mullani N, Gould L, Adler S, Krajewski K: Cerebral blood flow in chronic cocaine abusers: a study with positron emission tomography. Br J Psychiatry 1988; 152:641–648 4. Strickland TL, Miller BL, Kowell A, Stein R: Neurobiology of cocaine-induced organic brain impairment: contributions from functional neuroimaging. Neuropsychol Rev 1998; 8:1–9 5. Kaufman MJ, Levin JM, Ross MH, Lange N, Rose SL, Kukes TJ, Mendelson JH, Lukas SE, Cohen BM, Renshaw PF: Cocaine-induced cerebral vasoconstriction detected in humans with magnetic resonance angiography. JAMA 1998; 279:376–380 6. Holman BL, Mendelson J, Garada B, Teoh SK, Hallgring E, Johnson KA, Mello NK: Regional cerebral blood flow improves with treatment in chronic cocaine polydrug users. J Nucl Med 1993; 34:723–727

Am J Psychiatry 158:4, April 2001

7. O’Malley S, Adamse M, Heaton RK, Gawin FH: Neuropsychological impairment in chronic cocaine abusers. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse 1992; 18:131–144 8. Jellinek E: Phases of alcohol addiction. Q J Stud Alcohol 1952; 13:673–684 9. Siegmund DO, Worsley KJ: Testing for a signal with unknown location and scale in a stationary Gaussian random field. Annals of Statistics 1995; 23:608–639 10. Friston KJ, Holmes A, Poline J-B, Price CJ, Frith CD: Detecting activations in PET and fMRI: levels of inference and power. Neuroimage 1996; 40:223–235 11. van Gorp WG, Wilkins JN, Hinkin CH, Moore LH, Hull J, Horner MD, Plotkin D: Declarative and procedural memory functioning in abstinent cocaine abusers. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999; 56: 85–89

545