Dominance rank, resource availability, and ... - Springer Link

16 downloads 150 Views 786KB Size Report
2 Department of Anthropology, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093, USA and .... on captive and wild primates to suggest how rank might.
Behav Ecol Sociobiol (1993) 33:313-318

Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology © Springer-Verlag 1993

Dominance rank, resource availability, and reproductive maturation in female savanna baboons Fred B. Bercovitch 1, Shirley C. Strum 2 1 Caribbean Primate Research Center, University of Puerto Rico - Medical Sciences Campus, P.O. Box 1053, Sabana Seca, PR 00952, USA 2 Department of Anthropology, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093, USA and Institute of Primate Research, P.O. Box 24481, Karen, Nairobi, Kenya Received September 24, 1992 / Accepted after revision July 3, 1993

Summary. Ten years of data collected from a population of savanna baboons, Papio cynoeephalus anubis, residing near Gilgil, Kenya were analyzed to ascertain the extent to which social and ecological factors influence reproductive maturation in females. First sexual swelling occurred at an average age of 4.79 years and first birth occurred at an average age of 6.92 years. Age at first menses was significantly correlated with age at first sexual swelling, but age at first sexual swelling was not a good predictor of age at first birth. The amount of rainfall in the 6 months preceding first sexual swelling and resource availability were significantly correlated with age at first sexual swelling. When ecological factors were taken into account, dominant females had an earlier age at onset of puberty, but not an earlier age at first birth, than did subordinate females. We suggest that nutritional and social stress operate at the same physiological level to disrupt GnRH pulsatility and retard reproductive maturation in some females. Given that socioecological variables modify the timing of life history events related to fitness in female baboons, the task for the future is to unravel how socioecological factors influence different life history components and generate variation in lifetime reproductive success. Key words: Dominance - Puberty - First birth - Savanna baboons - Food availability Nest

Introduction Length of the reproductive lifespan is the main factor responsible for variation in lifetime reproductive success among a number of vertebrates (Clutton-Brock 1988; Newton 1989). One means of potentially increasing reproductive lifespan is to reduce age at production of first offspring. Yet age at first birth is not correlated with lifetime reproductive success in elephant seals, Mirounga anoustrisos, (LeBoeuf and Reiter 1988), Japanese maCorrespondence to: F.B. Bercovitch

caques, Macacafuseata, (Fedigan et al. 1986), or rhesus macaques, M. mulatta (Bercovitch and Berard 1993). The tempo of reproductive maturation appears to be partly a function of ecological conditions. For example, among wild horses, Equus caballus, rapid reproductive maturation characterizes individuals residing in better quality home ranges (Berger 1982). Cheney et al. (1988) reported a trend towards lower age at first birth in areas of better quality in three troops of vervet monkeys, Cercopithecus aethiops. Menarche in captive savanna baboons occurs between 3 and 4 years of age (Gilbert and Gillman 1960; Glassman et al. 1984; Coelho 1985) or about 1-2 years earlier than among wild savanna baboons (Altmann et al. 1981; Strum and Western 1982; Scott 1984). Savanna baboons within a population demonstrate a facultative response to resource availability by augmenting weight and fat under more plentiful conditions (Strum 1991 ; Altmann et al. 1993). When foods are clumped or restricted in distribution, high-ranking female primates tend to have a greater reproductive success (Harcourt 1987; Silk 1987; Bercovitch 1991). Dominant female primates have priority of access to limited food items, often monopolizing critical resources (Whitten 1983; Iwamoto 1987; Barton 1989). Because enhanced nutrition boosts growth rates and improves body condition (Strum 1991), we would expect that rank effects on female reproductive maturation would be most likely to occur under conditions of restricted or seasonal food distribution or availability. Three studies of food-enhanced primate populations have found that high-ranking females are more likely to reproduce at a young age than are their lower-ranking peers. Among rhesus macaques in the Cayo Santiago population (Bercovitch and Berard 1993) and living at La Parguera (Drickamer 1974), high-rank females are more likely to have their first offspring at 3 years of age than are low-rank females. Food provisioning accelerates age at first birth among high-ranking female Japanese macaques, but not among low-ranking females (Sugiyama and Ohsawa 1982). A rank-related acceleration in age at first birth among female primates is often assumed to

314 be a h a r b i n g e r o f greater lifetime r e p r o d u c t i v e success ( D r i c k a m e r 1974; Sade 1991), b u t d a t a collected over 30 years f r o m the C a y o S a n t i a g o p o p u l a t i o n c o n t r a d i c t this a s s u m p t i o n (Bercovich a n d Berard 1993). I n o n e o f few reports available f r o m u n p r o v i s i o n e d p r i m a t e p o p u l a tions, A l t m a n n et al. (1988) f o u n d t h a t d o m i n a n t female s a v a n n a b a b o o n s u n d e r w e n t p u b e r t y a n d h a d their first b i r t h at y o u n g e r ages t h a n s u b o r d i n a t e females, b u t they did n o t assess the effect o f resource availability o n sexual maturation. I n this r e p o r t we scrutinize b o t h social a n d ecological factors t h a t affect the t i m i n g o f r e p r o d u c t i v e m a t u r a t i o n i n a wild p o p u l a t i o n o f primates. W e identify h o w food a v a i l a b i l i t y c a n alter the t i m i n g o f life-history events a n d demonstrate how a variable environment can obviate differences in c o m p e t i t i v e ability. W e e x p a n d u p o n a n earlier analysis o f r e p r o d u c t i v e characteristics o f female s a v a n n a b a b o o n s ( S t r u m a n d W e s t e r n 1982) a n d link o u r findings f r o m the field with physiological d a t a o b t a i n e d o n captive a n d wild p r i m a t e s to suggest h o w r a n k m i g h t influence r e p r o d u c t i v e m a t u r a t i o n via a c o m m o n p a t h w a y u n d e r l y i n g n u t r i t i o n a l a n d social stress.

Methods Subjects and study location. The study subjects were born into Pumphouse troop, a social unit of baboons that has been the subject of behavioral observations almost continuously since 1970 (Strum 1987). The study site is about 1800 m above sea level near the town of Gilgil (0°30, S.) in the Central African Rift Valley of Kenya, 115 km northwest of Nairobi. All members of the troop were completely habituated to human observers and were recognized individually. Demographic information was compiled into the Gilgil Baboon Project Records. The information included daily census records on the reproductive cycle state of all females and birth dates, usually known to within a few days. Data analyzed in this report were collected between October 1971 and August 1981, but the key analyses are restricted to 1975-1981 due to the length of time required for female baboons to become adolescent. Birth dates were known for 16 of 19 females who traversed puberty between 1975 and 1981. The remaining three subjects have been assigned a birthdate corresponding to the first day of the month in which they were born. Births occur throughout the year at Gilgil (Bercovitch and Harding, in prep.). Between 1971 and 1981, troop size has ranged from about 50 to over 100 individuals, with between 18 and 23 adult females present at any one time. The study site is an open country, patchy scrub environment punctuated by nearly parallel outcroppings of volcanic cliffs. The flora is largely grassland, dotted with groves of acacia (Acacia • xanthophloea, A. drepanolobium, A. seyal), sandpaper (Cordia ovalis), and grewia (Grewia similis) trees, thickets of leleshwa bushes (Tarehonanthus camphoratus), and clumps of succulent plants (Opuntia spp., Aloe spp.). The baboons drank from cattle troughs, broken pipes, or temporary ponds and puddles. A variety of large mammals inhabited the troop's home range, including zebra (Equus burchelli), eland (Taurotragus oryx), Thomson's gazelle (Gazella thomsonii), impala (Aepycerus melampus), warthogs (Phacochoerus aetheiopicus), and domestic cattle and sheep. Poisonous snakes (puff adders, Bitis arietus; Egyptian cobra, Naja nigricollis) were regularly encountered and spotted hyena (Crocuta croeuta), were heard on occasion. Cheetah (Acinonyx jubatus) were sporadically sighted and left alone, but leopards (Panthera pardus) that were sighted were removed to protect domestic stock. Rainfall patterns vary from year to year, but the study site generally has a long rainy season (April to June), a short rainy

season (around November), and two intervening dry seasons. Annual variability in temperature is limited (Harding 1976).

Menstrual cycles in savanna baboons. Female savanna baboons have a perineal sexual skin that increases in size during the follicular phase of the menstrual cycle (Zuckerman 1930; Zuckerman and Parkes 1932; Gillman and Gilbert 1946). Swelling of the sexual skin is estrogen-dependent, but not necessarily indicative of ovulation (Zuckerman 1930; Zuckerman and Parkes 1932; Saayman 1972; Gillman and Gilbert 1946), and nulliparous female baboons have prolonged and irregular menstrual cycles (Scott 1984). Currently available techniques are inadequate to ascertain age at first ovulation in wild populations of primates. Menstruation in savanna baboons is visible to human observers, but it is not nearly as noticeable as are sexual swellings. Age at first sexual swelling, but not age at first menses, is available from a number of savanna baboon research sites, but many laboratory investigations of nonhuman primates use age at first menses to designate the beginning of puberty. When possible, we examine both life-history traits. We operationally define the onset of puberty as the day of first sexual swelling because (a) this is the first external sign of an increase in systemic estrogen associated with female adolescent development, (b) it is a common feature used in studies of wild baboons, and (c) it provides for a more in-depth analysis of a larger sample. Dominance rank and resource availability. Dominance hierarchies among female savanna baboons are extremely stable over a number of years (Hausfater et al. 1982; Samuels et al. 1987; Strum 1987). Daughters usually occupy a rank position immediately below that of their mothers, with older daughters subordinate to younger daughters and immature daughters of high-ranking females dominant to adult females of low rank. Nulliparous females were classified as high-, middle-, or low-ranking based upon their mothers' relative status as determined from the agonistic interactions that were contained within the long-term data base. Between 1971 and 1981, no profound alterations occurred in the female dominance hierarchy, indicating that mother's relative position within the dominance hierarchy was a reliable guide to relative rank of daughter. Resource availability was assessed in two ways. First, rainfall measurements were taken almost daily from a rain gauge located near the edge of the home range of Pumphouse troop. Although baboons have a very diverse diet, the major component of the diet among the Pumphouse troop is grass (Harding 1976). The amount of rainfall reflects primary productivity in African grassland environments (Walter 1973 ; Whittaker 1975). Food competition tends to be most severe among baboons at the end of the dry season (Post et al. 1980). Adult females in Pumphouse troop spent significantly more time feeding, and less time in social behavior, during the dry season (Bercovitch 1983). We have used the amount of rainfall in the 6 months prior to first sexual swelling as a means of gauging resource availability during the probable growth spurt in females. Among captive rhesus macaques, a limited growth spurt precedes menarche by about 2-6 months (Tanner et al. 1990). A pronounced growth spurt is not characteristic of wild female savanna baboons (Altmann and Alberts 1987; Strum 1991), but a growth spurt has been documented in captive female savanna baboons (Glassman et al. 1984). The second index of resource availability used was the food competition index, or FCI (Strum and Western 1982). Whereas rainfall provides an estimate of forage production, the resources actually available to baboons are less than this. As noted earlier, the savanna baboons at Gilgil are part of a wildlife community and share their ecosystem with many browzing and grazing ungulates that compete for the same food items. The FCI adjusts for both inter- and intra-specific food competition and yields a value estimating food availability to baboons in lieu of actual measurements of baboon food items in the environment (see Strum and Western 1982 for details). Estimates of ungulate grazing pressure and the impact

315 of conspecifics (i.e., troop size and home range area) are incorporated into the FCI. In sum, the FCI estimates the trough in food availability over the year while rainfall in the previous 6 months estimates the peak in food availability in the period immediately preceding the onset of puberty.

6.00

3O 25

(Z
0.50).

Discussion

Greater food availability and more rainfall were associated with a younger age at first sexual swelling, with rank effects superimposed upon ecological variables. Daughters of high-ranking females had accelerated onset of reproductive maturation only when resource availability was taken into account. However, rank effects on production of first offspring were not evident even when resource availability was considered. Although age at first birth was independent of dominance rank among the Gilgil baboons, age at first birth was younger among high-ranking female baboons at Amboseli Park compared to low-ranking females (Altmann et al. 1988). We suggest that the most likely explanation for the discrepancy between study locations resides in differential resource availability at the two sites. Amboseli is an arid environment with a mean annual rainfall of 280 mm (1973-1981; Western, pers. comm.), while mean annual rainfall at Gilgil is 700 mm. The more arid environment at Amboseli is characterized by extreme seasonality, periods of low biomass, and patchy food distribution of some important dietary elements, e.g., Acacia trees. The restricted distribution of food items in terms of both season and location creates conditions that are conducive to resource monopolization by higher-ranking females. Such patterns of resource availability and differential acquisition foster rank-related differences in reproductive output (Harcourt 1987; Bercovitch 1991). In contrast,.even when biomass is at its lowest at Gilgil, baboon food items are dispersed and indefensible,

yielding a situation that partly ameliorates rank-related effects on age at first birth. Differences reported between locations in Africa in the effect of rank on the timing of first birth may be the result of differences in habitat characteristics and the potential for resource monopolization by dominant individuals. Comparative data on the timing of reproductive maturation among savanna baboons is available from several research sites in Africa. Ransom (1981) suggested that some females had first sexual swellings at 3.5 years at the Gombe Reserve (Tanzania), but Packer (1979) reported a range from 4.5 years to 5.6 years at the same site, with a median age for onset of cycling of 4.96 years. At Amboseli National Park (Kenya), the onset of puberty is from 4.0 to 5.75 years, with an average age at first sexual swelling of 4.8 years (Altmann et al. 1981). The data from Gilgil are virtually identical to the data from Amboseli (range: 4.1-5.5 years; average=4.8 years). First birth at all three field sites usually occurs between 6.5 and 7.0 years (Packer 1979; Altmann et al. 1981, 1988; this study), but, in very rare cases, repeated menstrual cycling occurs without successful production of offspring (Hausfater 1975; Strum, unpubl, data). Among. the 60 females who have been observed in Pumphouse troop since 1970, only one has failed to produce offspring. For a given female savanna baboon, the timing of reproductive maturation will be molded by variation in ecological conditions, and constrained by physiological mechanisms. The earliest age at onset of puberty among wild female savanna baboons is about the same as the latest age at onset of puberty among captive baboons. Growth patterns and physical condition of female baboons at Gilgil under sustained plentiful conditions are essentially identical to those of captive female baboons (Strum 1991). The data from field and laboratory studies of savanna baboons concur in pinpointing the upper and lower bounds for life history events associated with reproductive maturation. Depending upon resource availability, the onset of puberty has a floor of about 3 years and a ceiling of about 6 years. No baboons have been reported to exceed this age without commencement of menstrual periods. The age at first birth has a floor of about 5 years, but no ceiling effect. Hence, regardless of food availability, eventually all females sustain first sexual swelling, but some females are barren throughout life. Puberty in primates commences with the appropriate timing of hypothalamic release of GnRH triggering regular pulsatile outbursts of gonadotropins (Plant 1988; Terasawa et al. 1983; Wilson 1989). Gonzalotropia releasing hormone (GnRH) is a polypeptide produced by th e hypothalamus and which provokes the anterior pitvitary to discharge luteinizing hormone (LH) and follicle stimulating hormone (FSH) into the circulation. The ratio of these two gonzalotropias, as well as their pulse frequency and amplitude, regulate ovulation. The pulsatile activity of GnRH is modulated by both nutritional (i.e., food type, quality, and quantity) and metabolic (i.e., food utilization and activity level) factors in captive primates (Cameron 1989). Severe food restriction has a strong suppressive effect on GnRH pulsatility and enhances negative feedback sensitivity to estradiol (Dubey

317 et al. 1986; Bronson 1989). Among male baboons in Kenya, testosterone concentrations during a drought declined by about 60% (Sapolsky 1986) and estradiol is a metabolite of testosterone (Martin 1985). Social factors also interfere with G n R H output in nonhuman primates. In captive marmosets, Callithrix jacchus, reproductive suppression of subordinate individuals is associated with stifled hypothalamic G n R H release and altered sensitivity to gonadal steroids (Abbott et al. 1988, 1990). Cortisol is the major glucocorticoid released in nohuman primates in response to stressful situations, and hyperactivity of the C R H - A C T H - g l u c o corticoid axis is linked with reproductive disorders, including depressed G n R H secretion, nonpulsatility of LH, and anovulation (Hagino 1972; Moberg et al. 1982; Martin 1985; Bronson 1989). In captive rhesus macaques, high-ranking females ovulate at younger ages than low-ranking females in the absence of differences in feeding time (Schwartz et al. 1985), and females who have their first ovulation at relatively young ages have lower cortisol levels than do peers who have their first ovulation at later ages (Schwartz, pers. comm.). The precursor to both cortisol and estradiol is pregnenolone (Martin 1985), so substrate limitations favoring enhanced production of corticosteroids would be likely to decrease production of sex steroids. It seems likely that reduced food availability arising from subordinate social status delays the onset of puberty among wild primates by interfering with G n R H profiles. Both nutritional stress and social stress are key entities influencing primate reproductive success (Dunbar (1988), but disentangling the impact of each factor may prove extremely difficult because both factors may interact under specific conditions in the wild. Inadequate nutrition and subordinate social status can both have a negative impact on reproduction due to the same physiological mechanism. F r o m an evolutionary perspective, age at first birth has a greater impact on fitness than does age at onset of puberty (Rowell 1977), but lengthy primate lifespans hinder collection of data on the relationship between the timing of these two life-history events. Our data indicated that age at first birth was not predictable on the basis of age at onset of puberty, whereas Altmann (1991) found that age at onset of puberty was predictive of age at first conception in the Amboseli population (r = 0.939, n = 5 , P < 0 . 0 5 ; from Table 1). In captive hamadryas baboons, P. hamadryas, neither age at onset of sexual swelling nor age at first birth were determined by dominance rank and early puberty was not associated with a younger age at first birth (Chalyan et al. 1991). A similar disassociation between the timing of first sexual swelling and age at onset of reproduction has been found among wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes (Pusey 1990), and first menses is not predictive of first ovulation in captive rhesus macaques (Schwartz et al. 1988; Bercovitch and G o y 1990). First birth is affected by the number of adolescent cycles, length of cycles, probability of ovulation, likelihood of conception, and chances o f early abortion. We suggest that socioecological variables directly influence neuroendocrine mechanisms responsible for reproductive cycling in females and that critical variables influenc-

ing the onset of reproductive maturation are likely to differ from those determing the age at first reproduction and the length o f the interbirth interval. Despite the evidence indicating that both dominance rank and resource availability influence the timing of reproductive events in a female's lifetime, we still confront large difficulties in deciphering how dominance rank impacts variation in female fitness. Advancements in this area will depend upon developing models integrating complex interactions among physiology, nutrition, longevity, ecology, and dominance status, as well as collecting longitudinal data to test the models.

Acknowledgements. We thank R.S.O. Harding, D. Harding, W. Malmi, H. Gilmore, P. Gilmore, L. Scott, M. Demment, D. Manzolillo, D. Bercovitch, J. Musau, D. Forthman, and H. Quick for contributing data to the Gilgil Baboon Project. Research has been funded by grants from the NSF, NIMH, L.S.B. Leakey Foundation, and the University of California. Occupancy at Kekopey Ranch was permitted through the graciousnessof A. and T. Cole and R. Dansie. Local sponsorship has been provided by the National Museums of Kenya and the Institute of Primate Research. Current financial support is provided by NIH Grant RR03640 to the Caribbean Primate Research Center (FBB), the L.S.B. Leakey Foundation (SCS), and Wildlife Conservation International (SCS). We appreciate the comments and suggestions made by R.I.M. Dunbar, R.S.O. Harding, S.M. Schwartz, and T.E. Rowell.

References Abbott DH, Hodges JK, George LM (1988) Social status controls LH secretion and ovulation in female marmoset monkeys (Callithrixjacchus). J Endocrinol 117:32%339 Abbott DH, George LM, Barrett J, Hodges JK, O'Byrne KT, Sheffield JW, Suterland IA, Chambers GR, Lunn SF, Ruiz de Elvira M-C (1990) Social control of ovulation in marmoset monkeys: a neuroendocrine basis for the study of infertility. In: Ziegler TE, Bercovitch FB (eds) Socioendocrinology of primate reproduction. Wiley-Liss, New York, pp 135-158 Altmann J, Alberts S (1987) Body mass and growth rates in a wild primate population~ Oecologia 72: 15-20 Altmann J, Altmann S, Hausfater G (1981) Physical maturation and age estimates of yellow baboons, Papio cynocephalus, in Amboseli National Park, Kenya. Am J Primatol 1: 389-399 Altmann J, Hausfater G, Altmann S (1988) Determinants of reproductive success in savannah baboons, Papio cynocephalus. In: Clutton-Brock TH (ed) Reproductive success. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 403-418 Altmann J, Schoeller D, Altmann SA, Muruthi P, Sapolsky RM (1993) Body size and fatness of free-living baboons reflect food availability and activity levels. Am J Primatol 30:149-161 Altmann S (1991) Diets of yearling female primates (Papio cynocephalus) predict lifetime fitness. Proc Natl Acad Sci 88:420~23 Barton RA (1989) Foraging strategies, diet and competition in olive baboons (Papio anubis). Ph.D. thesis, University of St Andrews, Scotland Bercovitch FB (1983) Time budgets and consortships in olive baboons (Papio anubis). Folia Primatol 41:180-190 Bercovitch FB (1991) Social stratification, social strategies, and reproductive success in primates. Ethol Sociobiol 12:315-333 Bercovitch FB, Berard JD (1993) Life history costs and consequences of rapid reproductivematuration in femalerhesus macaques. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 32:103-109 Bercovitch FB, Goy RW (1990) The socioendocrinology of reproductive development and reproductive success in macaques. In: Ziegler TE, Bercovitch FB (eds) Socioendocrinology of primate reproduction. Wiley-Liss, New York, pp 59-93 Berger J (1982) Wild horses of the Great Basin. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

318 Bronson FH (1989) Mammalian reproductive biology. University of Chicago Press, Chicago Cameron JL (1989) Nutritional and metabolic determinants of GnRH secretion in primate species. In: Delemarre.van de Waal HA, Plant TM, Rees GP van, Schoemaker J (eds) Control of the onset of puberty III. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 275-284 Chalyan VG, Meishvili NV, Dathe R (1991) Dominance rank and reproduction in female hamadryas baboons. Primate Rep 29:35-40 Cheney DL, Seyfarth EM, Andelman SJ, Lee PC (1988) Reproductive success in vervet monkeys. In: Clutton-Brock TH (ed) Reproductive success. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 384-402 Clutton-Brock TH (ed) (1988) Reproductive success. University of Chicago Press, Chicago Coelho AM Jr (1985) Baboon dimorphism: growth in weight, length and adiposity from birth to 8 years of age. In: Watts ES (ed) Nonhuman primaate models for human growth and development. Alan R. Liss, New York, pp 125-159 Drickamer LC (1974) A ten-year summary of reproductive data for free-ranging Macaca mulatta. Folia primatol 21:61-80 Dubey AK, Cameron JL, Steiner RA, Plant TM (1986) Inhibition of gonadotropin secretion in castrated male rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) induced by dietary restriction: analogy with the prepubertal hiatus of gonodotropin release. Endocrinology 118:518-525 Dunbar RIM (1988) Primate social systems. Cornell University Press, Ithaca Fedigan LM, Fedigan L, Gouzoules S, Gouzoules H, Koyama N (1986) Lifetime reproductive success in female Japanese macaques. Folia Primatol 47:143-157 Gilbert C, Gillman J (1960) Puberty in the baboon (Papio ursinus) in relation to age and body weight. S Afr J Med Sci 25:99-103 Gillman J, Gilbert C (1946) The reproductive cycle of the chacma baboon (Papio ursinus) with special reference to the problems of menstrual irregularities as assessed by the behaviour of the sex skin. S Afr J Med Sci Suppl 11 : 1-54 Glassman DM, Coelho AM jr, Carey KD, Bramblett CA (1984) Weight growth in savannah baboons: a longitudinal study from birth to adulthood. Growth 48:425-433 Hagino N (1972) The effect of snythetic corticosteroids on ovarian function in the baboon. Endocrinology 35:716-721 Harcourt AH (1987) Dominance and fertility among female primates. J Zoo1 213:471-487 Harding RSO (1976) Ranging patterns of a troop of baboons (Papio anubis) in Kenya. Folia Primatol 25:143-185 Hausfater G (1975) Dominance and reproduction in baboons. Karger, Basel Hausfater G, Altmann J, Altmann S (1982) Long-term consistency of dominance relations among female baboons (Papio cynocephalus). Science 217: 752-755 Iwamoto T (1987) Feeding strategies of primates in relation to social status. In: Ito Y, Brown JL, Nikkawa J (eds) Animal societies: theories and facts. Japan Sci Soc Press, Tokyo, pp 243-252 Le Boeuf BJ, Reiter J (1988) Lifetime reproductive success in northem elephant seals. In: Clutton-Brock TH (ed) Reproductive success. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 344-362 Martin C (1985) Endocrine physiology. Oxford University Press, Oxford Moberg GP, Watson JG, Hayashi KT (1982) Effects of adrenocorticotropin treatment on estrogen, luteinizing hormone, and progesterone in the female rhesus monkey. J Med Primatol 11 : 235-241 Newton I (ed) (1989) Lifetime reproductive success in birds. Academic, San Diego Packer C (1979) Inter-troop transfer and inbreeding avoidance in Papio anubis. Anim Behav 27:1 36 Plant TM (1988) Puberty in primates. In: Knobil E, Neill J (eds) The physiology of reproduction. Raven, New York, pp 1763-1788

Post DG, Hausfater G, McCusky SA (1980) Feeding behavior of yellow baboons (Papio cynocephalus): relationship to age, gender and dominance rank. Folia Primatol 34:170-195 Pusey AE (1990) Behavioural changes at adolescence in chimpanzees. Behaviour 115: 203-246 Ransom TW (1981) Beach troop of the Gombe. Bucknell University Press, Lewisburg Rowell TE (1977) Variation in age at puberty in monkeys. Folia Primatol 27: 284-296 Saayman GS (1972) Effects of ovarian hormones upon the sexual skin and mounting behaviour in the free-ranging chacma baboon (Papio ursinus). Folia Primatol 17:297-303 Sade DS (1991) Intrapopulation variation in life-history parameters. In: DeRosseau CJ (ed) Primate life history and evolution. Wiley-Liss, New York, pp 181-194 Samuels A, Silk JB, Altmann J (1987) Continuity and change in dominance relations among female baboons. Anim Behav 35:785-793 Sapolsky RM (1986) Endocrine and behavioral correlates of drought in wild olive baboons (Papio anubis). Am J Primatol 11 : 217-227 Schwartz SM, Wilson ME, Walker ML, Collins DC (1985) Social and growth correlaties of puberty onset in female rhesus monkeys. Nutr Behav 2:225-232 Schwartz SM, Wilson ME, Walker ML, Collins DC (1988) Dietary influences on growth and sexual maturation in premenarchial rhesus monkeys. Horm Behav 22:231-251 Scott LM (1984) Reproductive behavior of adolescent female baboons (Papio anubis) in Kenya. In: Small MF (ed) Female primates: studies by women primatologists, Alan R. Liss, New York, pp 77-100 Silk JB (1987) Social behavior in evolutionary perspective. In: Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW, Struhsaker TT (eds) Primate societies. Chicago, University of Chicago Press, pp 318-329 Strum SC (1987) Almost human. Random House, New York Strum SC (1991) Weight and age in wild olive baboons. Am J Primatol 25: 219-237 Strum SC, Western JD (1982) Variations in fecundity with age and environment in olive baboons (Papio anubis). Am J Primatol 3:61-76 Sugiyama Y, Ohsawa H (1982) Population dynamics of Japanese monkeys with special reference to the effect of artificial feeding. Folia Primatol 39: 238-263 Tanner JM, Wilson ME, Rudman CG (1990) Pubertal growth spurt in the female rhesus monkey: relation to menarche and skeletal maturation. Am J Hum Biol 2:101-106 Terasawa E, Nass TE, Yeoman RR, Louse MD, Schultz NJ (1983) Hypothalamic control of puberty in the female rhesus macaque. In: Norman RL (ed) Neuroendocrine aspects of reproduction. Academic, New York, pp 149-182 Walter H (1973) Vegetation of the earth in relation to climate and ecophysiological conditions. Springer, New York Whittaker R (1975) Communities and ecosystems. Macmillan, New York Whitten PL (1983) Diet and dominance among female vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops). Am J Primatol 5:139-159 Wilson ME (1989) Relationship between growth and puberty in the rhesus monkey. In: Dalemarre van de Waal HA, Plant TM, Rees GP van, Schoemaker J (eds) Control of the onset of puberty III. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 137-149 Zuckerman S (1930) The menstrual cycle of the primates. Part I. General nature and homology. Proc Zool Soc London 1930:691 754 Zuckerman S, Parkes AS (1932) The menstrual cycle of the primates. Part V. The cycle of the baboon. Proc Zool Soc London 1932:139-191 •