INTERPERSONAL COMMUNICATION AND ...

2 downloads 46395 Views 283KB Size Report
employed two recruitment strategies: (1) a direct mail (DM) campaign and (2) an ... intervention from the Sage Call Center (DM=870, ORC=4,550), with 2,456 ...
1

1

Running Head: INTERPERSONAL COMMUNICATION AND SMOKING CESSATION

2

Forthcoming in Journal of Health Communication

3 4 5 6

Interpersonal Communication and Smoking Cessation in the Context of an Incentive-Based Program: Survey Evidence from a Telehealth Intervention in a Low-Income Population

7 8

Michael J. Parks, PhD1

9

Jonathan S. Slater, PhD1,2

10

Alexander J. Rothman, PhD2

11

Christina L. Nelson, MPH1

12 13 14 15

1

Minnesota Department of Health, Saint Paul, MN, USA 2

University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA

16 17

Corresponding author: Michael J. Parks, Minnesota Department of Health, 85 East 7th Street,

18

Saint Paul, MN 55164, USA. Email: [email protected]. Phone: 651-201-5285.

19

Acknowledgements: Project funded through Centers for Disease Control and Prevention

20

(American Recovery and Reinvestment Act; Patient Protection and Affordable Care Act). We

21

thank QUITPLAN® Helpline staff, Shelly Madigan, Sage patient navigators, Janis Taramelli, and

22

Michelle Waste for their efforts. The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

23 24

Keywords: smoking cessation; financial incentives; low-income; telehealth; interpersonal communication; population-level interventions

25 26

2

27

Abstract

28

The tobacco epidemic disproportionately affects low-income populations, and telehealth is an

29

evidence-based strategy for extending tobacco cessation services to underserved populations. A

30

public health priority is to establish incentive-based interventions at the population level in order

31

to promote long-term smoking cessation in low-income populations. Yet randomized clinical

32

trials show that financial incentives tend to encourage only short-term steps of cessation, not

33

continuous smoking abstinence. One potential mechanism for increasing long-term cessation is

34

interpersonal communication (IPC) in response to population-level interventions. However, more

35

research is needed on IPC and its influence on health behavior change, particularly in the context

36

of incentive-based, population-level programs. This study used survey data gathered after a

37

population-level telehealth intervention that offered $20 incentives to low-income smokers for

38

being connected to Minnesota’s free quitline in order to examine how perceived incentive

39

importance and IPC about the incentive-based program relate to both short-term and long-term

40

health behavior change. Results show that IPC was strongly associated with initial quitline

41

utilization and continuous smoking abstinence as measured by 30-day point prevalence rates at

42

seven-month follow-up. Perceived incentive importance had weak associations with both

43

measures of cessation, and all associations were non-significant in models adjusting for IPC.

44

These results were found in descriptive analyses, logistic regression models, and Heckman probit

45

models that adjusted for participant recruitment. In sum, a behavioral telehealth intervention

46

targeting low-income smokers that offered a financial incentive inspired IPC, and this social

47

response was strongly related to utilization of intervention services as well as continuous

48

smoking abstinence.

49

3

50

Introduction

51

Tobacco use is the leading preventable cause of mortality and morbidity in the U.S. and

52

abroad (World Health Organization, 2012). Annually, smoking is estimated to be responsible for

53

five million deaths worldwide as it is causally linked to cardiovascular disease and multiple

54

forms of cancer (McAfee, Davis, Alexander, Pechacek, & Bunnell, 2013). The tobacco epidemic

55

disproportionally affects people of low socioeconomic status. Smoking prevalence among adults

56

below the federal poverty level is 28% whereas prevalence at or above poverty level is 17%

57

(CDC, 2014). Disproportionately high smoking rates persist among low-income women (Stewart

58

et al., 2010), and it is estimated that smoking accounts for up to half of male mortality disparities

59

associated with low socioeconomic status in countries such as the U.S. (Jha et al., 2006).

60

Consequently, increasing smoking cessation within low socioeconomic groups can save millions

61

of lives and decrease mortality disparities (Holford et al., 2014; Jha et al., 2006; Thomas et al.,

62

2008).

63

There is a critical need to develop population-level smoking cessation programs for low-

64

income populations that go beyond clinic-based settings as clinics have limited access to low-

65

income populations who underutilize preventive services, are geographically isolated, and

66

inadequately insured (see Chokshi & Farley, 2014; Bryant et al., 2011; Wilson, 1987).

67

Telehealth, or the use of “telecommunications and information technology to provide access to

68

health assessment, diagnosis, intervention, consultation, supervision, education, and information

69

across distance” (Nickelson, 1998: 527), is effective for delivering health services to underserved

70

and low-income populations (Wootton, Jebamani, & Dow, 2005; McBride & Rimer, 1999). Free

71

state telephone tobacco quitlines are exemplars of telehealth’s potential. Free telephone quitlines

4

72

are evidence-based techniques for increasing smoking abstinence rates, particularly in low-

73

income and non-White populations (Stead, Perera, & Lancaster, 2007; Burns, Deaton, &

74

Levinson, 2011; Fiore et al., 2008), but utilization rates are low across the U.S. (Zhu, Lee,

75

Zhuang, Gamst, & Wolfson, 2012).

76

Another promising strategy for extending health services to underserved populations is

77

financial incentives (Oliver, 2009). Designing incentive-based, population-level interventions in

78

order to sustain long-term changes in health behaviors like smoking cessation has become a

79

public health priority, particularly in low-income populations (Blumenthal et al., 2013). This is

80

exemplified by the Affordable Care Act’s section 4108 and the Centers for Medicare and

81

Medicaid Services’ authority to provide grants to states to test the effectiveness of incentives in

82

improving health behaviors such as tobacco cessation (Blumenthal et al., 2013: 497-498).

83

Financial incentives have been shown to increase health enhancing behaviors (e.g., Slater et al.,

84

2005; Gneezy, Meier, & Rey-Biel, 2011), and low-income smokers tend to be responsive to

85

incentives (Bryant et al., 2011). However, incentives tend to be effective for encouraging

86

preventive care that requires only a single activity or “simple” behavior but not “complex”

87

actions that require additional engagement beyond initial intervention services (see Kane,

88

Johnson, Town, & Butler, 2004). Smoking cessation requires prolonged engagement (Prochaska,

89

DiClemente, & Norcross, 1992), and while incentives can influence short-term cessation in the

90

context of a clinical trial (Volpp et al., 2009; Sigmon & Patrick, 2012) less is known about how

91

incentives can encourage or be used to promote long-term cessation in population-level

92

interventions within low-income populations (Blumenthal et al., 2013).

5

93

Research in behavioral economics demonstrates that financial incentives typically have

94

an effect on short-term behavior change through the “direct price effect” that makes incentivized

95

behaviors more attractive (Gneezy et al., 2011). The direct price effect is often explained by its

96

influence on extrinsic motivation to engage in a target behavior and it tends to be less effective

97

for promoting behavior maintenance once incentives are removed. A second type of effect,

98

known as the “indirect psychological effect,” can have both positive and negative influences on

99

incentivized behaviors (Gneezy et al., 2011: 11). Multiple social and psychological factors can

100

alter the effects of a financial incentive presentation in terms of promoting or unintentionally

101

discouraging the incentivized behavior (see e.g., Babcock, Bedard, Charness, Hartman, & Royer,

102

2011).

103

Two specific factors are particularly relevant for population-level interventions, and they

104

have implications for both short-term and long-term behavior change. The first is that the

105

incentive’s effect depends on the context within which it is presented. If presented in a private

106

context within a controlled environment (e.g., randomized clinical trials), social influences are

107

limited, such as informal social control or social image processes. Alternatively, incentives

108

offered in more public settings can activate social influences (Gneezy et al., 2011). Public

109

settings are marked by the presence of others, and this can influence individuals’ behavior by

110

inspiring social responses to incentives instead of strictly monetary responses. For example,

111

social reactions such as interpersonal discussions about the incentive can activate behavior based

112

on social image maintenance or prevailing social norms (Gneezy et al., 2011: 11). A second and

113

related factor is that incentives can interact with social networks. Evidenced by past research on

114

exercise maintenance, incentives can not only increase exercise, but social groups (e.g., friends)

115

who jointly receive incentives are more likely to exhibit long-term behavior maintenance

6

116

(Charness & Gneezy, 2009; Babcock et al., 2011). Addressing the relational context of health

117

behaviors through network-oriented strategies has become a priority for prevention programs

118

(Gest, Osgood, Feinberg, Bierman, & Moody, 2011), and this is especially true for smoking

119

cessation interventions because social dynamics operating within networks strongly influence the

120

cessation process (Christakis & Fowler, 2008; Valente, 2010; Burns, Rothman, Fu, Lindgren, &

121

Joseph, 2014). Yet less is known about how these social mechanisms may influence long-term

122

health behavior change in response to population-level interventions, and particularly incentive-

123

based interventions.

124

In sum, randomized clinical trials demonstrate that incentive-based interventions can

125

encourage short-term behavioral engagement involving an incentivized behavior, and past

126

behavioral economic research implies that financial incentives offered in population-level

127

interventions can potentially encourage social responses and indirect effects due to the context of

128

the intervention. Population-level interventions occur in relatively public settings in conjunction

129

with naturally occurring psychosocial dynamics, and these processes could have implications for

130

both short-term and long-term behavior change. In terms of long-term behavior change such as

131

sustained smoking cessation, a social response to an incentive-based program at the population-

132

level could be a marker of naturally occurring social mechanisms that have inherent capacity to

133

promote sustained behavioral engagement.

134

Past population-level smoking cessation interventions provide empirical support (see e.g.,

135

McAfee et al., 2013; Donovan, Boulter, Borland, Jalleh, & Carter, 2003; van den Putte, Yzer,

136

Southwell, de Bruijn, & Willemsen, 2011). Smoking cessation programs have demonstrated that

137

long-term smoking cessation is indirectly influenced by social reactions measured by

7

138

interpersonal communication about the smoking cessation program (van den Putte, et al., 2011).

139

Supported by communication theories (see Katz & Lazarsfeld, 1955; Southwell & Yzer, 2007),

140

interpersonal communication about a health promotion or prevention program can catalyze

141

mechanisms embedded within social interactions, potentially cultivating intentions to change

142

behavior, activating normative pressure, and making health behavior decisions more cognitively

143

salient (see e.g., Montano & Kasprzyk, 2008; Southwell, 2013). Yet further examination of

144

interpersonal communication and health behavior change in response to population-level

145

interventions remains an important gap in research, particularly in the context of incentive-based

146

programs (van den Putte, et al., 2011; Southwell, Slater, Nelson, & Rothman, 2012).

147

The Current Study

148

This paper reports results of a survey conducted after a population-level intervention that

149

offered financial incentives to low-income smokers for being connected to Minnesota’s free

150

QUITPLAN® Helpline. The survey captured self-report measures of interpersonal

151

communication about the incentive-based program as well as perceived importance of the

152

incentive for the incentivized behavior (i.e., being connected to a quitline). The analysis focuses

153

on the cessation process measured by short-term and long-term behavioral engagement. Using

154

North American Quitline Consortium (NAQC) evidence-based measures (see NAQC, 2011) the

155

cessation process is measured by (1) initial utilization of quitline services, and (2) continuous

156

smoking abstinence measured by 30 consecutive smoke-free days at seven-month follow-up. The

157

main focus of the analysis is on how these two measures of the cessation process relate to both

158

interpersonal communication and perceived incentive importance.

8

159

Based on past research on the effects of interpersonal communication in response to

160

smoking cessation interventions, we hypothesize that interpersonal communication in the context

161

of the current incentive-based telehealth intervention will be related to utilization of smoking

162

cessation services as well as subsequent efforts to quit smoking (i.e., seven-month follow-up 30-

163

day point prevalence rates). This hypothesis is also based on past research demonstrating that

164

incentives presented in more public settings can inspire social and psychological mechanisms,

165

and that these mechanisms are potentially conducive to long-term behavioral engagement. In

166

light of past research on financial incentives and smoking cessation in randomized clinical trials

167

as well as the direct price effect of incentives on incentivized behaviors, we note that incentives

168

are more likely to influence short-term behavior change rather than long-term smoking cessation.

169

Therefore, we hypothesize that perceived incentive importance will relate to initial steps of

170

cessation measured by utilization of quitline services, and that perceived incentive importance

171

will be less likely to impact continuous smoking abstinence.

172 173 174

Method Participants and Procedures Intervention context. From September 2010 to September 2012, a behavioral telehealth

175

intervention was implemented through Minnesota’s National Breast and Cervical Cancer Early

176

Detection Program (NBCCEDP) called “Sage.” Sage serves low-income individuals

177

experiencing health-related disparities. Specifically, Sage provides free cancer screening services

178

to individuals who are ages 40 years or older, have household incomes at or below 250% of the

179

federal poverty level, and are inadequately insured. Unique among NBCCEDPs, Sage has a call

180

center staffed by “patient navigators” (see Freund et al., 2008) who are fluent in English and

9

181

other languages relevant for Sage’s target population. More details on NBCCEDPs and Sage

182

have been published elsewhere (Lee et al., 2014; Slater et al., 2005).

183

Intervention. The current intervention offered a $20 incentive for being connected to

184

Minnesota’s QUITPLAN® Helpline (QL) via a three-way phone call conducted by Sage patient

185

navigators. Relying on participant-initiated phone contact (see Soet & Basch, 1997), we

186

employed two recruitment strategies: (1) a direct mail (DM) campaign and (2) an opportunistic

187

referral with connection (ORC) call transfer system from within the Sage Call Center.

188

Individuals identified as smokers in Sage’s database were recruited using DM mailers designed

189

to prompt cigarette smokers to call Sage’s toll-free phone number. The DM mailers consisted of

190

a folded card with emotionally evocative messages and graphics as well as a small insert card

191

advertising the financial incentive. The mailers were strategically constructed based on past

192

research (see Rothman & Salovey, 1997) and included two rounds of mailings (see Slater et al.,

193

2005).

194

For ORC recruitment, Sage’s patient navigators obtained the smoking status of

195

individuals who called the Sage Call Center for cancer screening information or appointments.

196

Callers identified as smokers were then opportunistically presented the QL referral offer (i.e., a

197

$20 incentive for being connected to the QL). Patient navigators handled phone calls for both

198

intervention groups, and once participants agreed to be transferred, patient navigators put

199

participants on hold and called QL operators. Patient navigators then confirmed with QL

200

operators that an individual from Sage would be connected to the QL via three-way calls. Once

201

QL operators agreed, patient navigators remained on the line until QL operators and intervention

10

202

participants were connected and communication between participants and QL operators was

203

confirmed.

204

QL services included free smoking cessation telephone counseling with a maximum of

205

five sessions within a six-month period. The QL provided self-help materials and free nicotine

206

replacement therapy for those who requested them. A total of 5,420 callers were offered the

207

intervention from the Sage Call Center (DM=870, ORC=4,550), with 2,456 completing QL

208

transfers. Of those transferred, 66% were ORC (N=1,612) compared to DM (N=844).

209

Survey data. Participants who completed QL connections were interviewed via

210

telephone by trained staff at least seven months after each participant’s QL connection in

211

compliance with recommended, evidence-based practices (see NAQC, 2011). All participants

212

who completed QL connections were targeted for the survey; 10 call attempts were made before

213

participants were deemed unresponsive. A total of 1,218 participants completed the survey.

214

Since not all participants were offered QL services after being connected by Sage patient

215

navigators, the current analyses focused on individuals who were offered QL services (N=995)

216

with complete data, comprising an analytic sample of 970. Individuals not offered services were

217

excluded because they were not subsequently asked about smoking cessation in the survey (e.g.,

218

utilization of QL services). No significant differences were found between individuals offered

219

and not offered services in terms of demographic and smoking characteristics, except for age

220

(M=53.7 vs. M=52.2, p=.01). Of those not offered services, about 40% reported it was due to

221

their insurance coverage, and 60% reported miscellaneous reasons associated with program

222

fidelity (e.g., accidental disconnection after patient navigator transfer, reported not receiving a

223

call back from QL).

11

224

Measures. Following past research, two dichotomous outcomes for short- and long-term

225

behavioral steps associated with smoking cessation were employed: (1) QL utilization and (2)

226

being smoke-free for 30 consecutive days at seven-month follow-up (see NAQC, 2011;

227

DiClement et al., 1991). For QL utilization, respondents were asked whether they had used the

228

tools and services that were offered from the QL (1=yes, 0=no). For continuous smoking

229

abstinence (“continuous cessation”), participants were asked if they had not smoked for 30

230

consecutive days (in interviews conducted at least seven months after being connected to the QL)

231

(1=yes; 0=no).

232

Incentive influence was captured by measuring whether participants reported the

233

incentive to be important for their QL connection (1=important, 0=not important).1 Interpersonal

234

communication about the incentive-based program was measured as whether or not participants

235

“told others about the Sage offer that rewards smokers $20 for being connected with a tobacco

236

quitline through the Sage Call Center” (1=yes, 0=no).

237

Background characteristics included age, sex, education, race/ethnicity, and smoking

238

history. Age was measured in continuous years; sex and race/ethnicity were dichotomous

239

measures (1=male, 0=female; 1=white, 0=non-white). Education was an ordinal scale, ranging

240

from 1 (completed 8th grade or less) to 6 (graduate school). Smoking characteristics included a

241

continuous measure of years smoked, whether or not participants smoked on a daily basis 1

Empirically, we tested the credibility of this measure by comparing answers to the question about how important the incentive was for the individual to an additional question that asked how important the incentive was for the Minnesota Department of Health to get smokers to connect to the QL in general. We created a scale out of the two measures, which had a reliability measure of .901. Results using this alternative measure were not different from main analyses that utilized the dichotomous measure, and therefore we only report the results for the dichotomous measure described here.

12

242

(1=yes, 0=no), whether they lived with a smoker (1=yes, 0=no), whether they made a quit

243

attempt in the past with medication (1=yes, 0=no), and whether they were unlikely to contact the

244

QL without the current intervention (1=yes, 0=no). Variables used to assess recruitment

245

differences were (1) a dichotomous measure for DM vs ORC, and (2) a measure of whether the

246

participant had past involvement with Sage (1=Sage, 0=non-Sage). Approximately 65% of the

247

analytic sample was recruited via ORC (ORC=629; DM=341), and 62% had previous contact

248

with Sage.

249

Analytic strategy. The first set of analyses of the analytic sample of surveyed

250

respondents consisted of descriptive statistics and cross-tabulations. These descriptive analyses

251

focused on bivariate relationships for (1) both cessation outcomes and interpersonal

252

communication, and (2) both cessation outcomes and incentive importance. We then explored

253

these relationships more systematically via logistic regression models for each dichotomous

254

outcome. It is important to note that DM and ORC recruitment groups likely differed in their

255

behavioral stage or “readiness” in terms of smoking cessation. This is because the DM group

256

called Sage seeking smoking cessation services whereas the ORC group called regarding

257

unrelated health matters and were unexpectedly offered cessation services. As a result, we ran

258

logistic regression models that included a dummy variable for DM versus ORC group

259

membership, as well as supplementary analyses that consisted of a sub-sample analysis that

260

adjusted for differences between DM and ORC groups in terms of their likelihood of group

261

membership (see Heckman, 1979; Bushway, Johnson, & Slocum, 2007).

262 263

The supplementary analysis consisted of maximum likelihood bivariate probit with selection models (Heckman probit models). In Heckman probit models, recruitment groups were

13

264

used as a bivariate selection criterion (DM vs. ORC)—“outcome” models in Heckman probit

265

models censored unobserved objects from the “selection” model (i.e., ORC individuals). This

266

generated an analysis of QL utilization and 30-day point prevalence quit status for a subgroup

267

potentially more prepared for smoking cessation (i.e., DM) while adjusting for the likelihood of

268

DM membership (see Puhani, 2000; Bushway et al., 2007). Outcome models are presented in

269

table form and within the text, but selection models are not reported (selection models included

270

all variables in outcome models except for an exclusion restriction variable). Multivariate

271

logistic regression and Heckman probit models were run in Stata, version 12. Cluster-adjusted

272

robust standard errors were used in both logistic regression and Heckman probit models in order

273

to account for clustering within patient navigators as well as for heteroscedasticity (Bushway et

274

al., 2007).

275 276 277

Results Participants Participant characteristics are detailed in Table 1. The analytic sample was

278

overwhelmingly low-income female smokers because low-income, inadequately insured women

279

are Sage’s target population. It included a limited number of males primarily as a result of

280

information sharing about the intervention.

281

The sample was primarily White (75%); 15% were African American, 5% were Native

282

American, 2% were Hispanic, and the remaining were other races or ethnicities (3%), providing

283

evidence that the intervention disproportionately reached racial and ethnic minorities in

284

Minnesota (U.S. Census Bureau, 2012), which is also Sage’s target population. Average

14

285

education level was a high school diploma or equivalent. About 12% of the sample did not

286

complete high school and all individuals met income guidelines for receiving Sage services,

287

reflecting the relatively low socioeconomic status of the sample.

288

The majority of participants were intense smokers. Years smoked was normally

289

distributed with an average of 30.94. Over 50% had made a quit attempt with medication in the

290

past, 94% smoked every day, and about 38% lived with a smoker. Most participants were

291

unlikely to have contacted a QL without having been reached with the current intervention

292

(72%).

293 294 295

[TABLE 1 ABOUT HERE] Survey Results Of the surveyed participants who were offered QL services, 643 individuals (66%) were

296

connected to the QL and utilized the services, such as receiving telephone counseling or self-help

297

materials. In terms of 30-day point prevalence quit rates, about 19% reported continuous

298

cessation seven months after QL connection (184 individuals). About 50% engaged in

299

interpersonal communication about the incentive-based program (IPC), and almost 69% noted

300

the incentive was important for their QL connection (see Table 1).

301

QL utilization. The associations between QL utilization and IPC, and between utilization

302

and incentive importance, are displayed in the left-hand portion of Table 2. Interpersonal

303

communication about the incentive-based program was associated with QL utilization (2=32.32;

304

p