Host plant selection by larvae of the muga silk

0 downloads 0 Views 1MB Size Report
Dec 4, 2011 - pebrine spores, and eggs produced by females that were free .... was stopped. The amount of ... the moment the insect started walking upwind.
Journal of Insect Science: Vol. 13 | Article 52

Bora et al.

Host plant selection by larvae of the muga silk moth, Antheraea assamensis, and the role of the antenna and maxillary palp D. S. Bora1a*, B. Deka1b, and A. Sen2c 1

Department of Life Sciences, Dibrugarh University, Dibrugarh-786004, Assam, India. National Chemical Laboratory, Pune, MH, India

2

Abstract The importance of olfactory senses in food preference in fifth instar larvae of Antheraea assamensis Helfer (Lepidoptera: Saturniidae) was examined by subjecting larvae with only antennae or maxillary palpi after microsurgery to food and odor choice tests. Mean percent consumption, total consumption, and choice indices were used as parameters for drawing conclusions. The foods used were two hosts, two non-hosts, and a neutral medium (water). Both antennae and maxillary palpi were fully competent in preference for host plants, Persea bombycina Kostermans (Laurales: Lauraceae) and Litsea polyantha Juss, over the non-hosts, Litsea grandifolia Teschner and Ziziphus jujuba Miller (Rosales: Rhamnaceae). Both were competent in rejecting the non-hosts, L. grandifolia and Z. jujuba. The odor choice test was carried out using a Y-tube olfactometer and showed similar results to the ingestive tests. The results indicate the necessity of functional integration of a combination of olfactory and gustatory sensilla present in different peripheral organs in food acceptance by A. assamensis larvae.

Abbreviations: ALL, larvae retaining all chemosensory organs; ANT, larvae retaining only antennae; MAX, larvae retaining only maxillary palpi; NONE, larvae retaining none of the chemosensory organs; UNI, larvae retaining all organs unilaterally Keywords: food choice, maxillae, Y-tube olfactometer Correspondence: a [email protected], b [email protected], c [email protected], *Corresponding author Editor: Jurgen Ziesmann was editor of this paper. Received: 4 December 2011 Accepted: 3 September 2012 Copyright : This is an open access paper. We use the Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 license that permits unrestricted use, provided that the paper is properly attributed. ISSN: 1536-2442 | Vol. 13, Number 52 Cite this paper as: Bora DS, Deka B, Sen A. 2013. Host plant selection by larvae of the muga silk moth, Antheraea assamensis, and the role of the antenna and maxillary palp. Journal of Insect Science 13:52. Available online: http://www.insectscience.org/13.52

Journal of Insect Science | www.insectscience.org

             

       1

Journal of Insect Science: Vol. 13 | Article 52 Introduction During foraging, herbivorous insects perceive different information available in the natural environment, and a specialist might use specific biophysical and chemical cues provided by a host plant. Insects use various sensory systems to locate their hosts (Chapman 2003; Bullas-Appleton et al. 2004; Heisswolf et al. 2007; Jorgensen et al. 2007), and the degree of acceptability is probably based on the ability of the plants to elicit qualitatively different perceptions because of high chemosensory selectivity of output neurons (Dethier and Crnjar 1982; Reisenman et al. 2005). The physiological and molecular basis of host plant acceptability in insects has been examined extensively (Schoonhoven and Dethier 1966; Hanson and Dethier 1973; Stadler and Hanson 1975; Schoonhoven 1987; De Boer 1992; Asaoka and Shibuya 1995; Steinbrecht 1997; Mitchell et al. 1999; Ting et al. 2002; Shiraiwa 2008; Riffel et al. 2009). However, insects have a wide range of diet-breadth and, considering the phenomenon of chemosensory tuning to host-recognition cues (Glendinning et al. 2000; Del Campo and Miles 2003) and preferences for one specific compound present in all members of one plant family leading to oligophagy (Del Campo et al. 2001), every insect species may be considered to have its own key (Schoonhoven and Loon 2002). While it is well documented that the gustatory system plays a leading role in food discrimination (Hanson and Dethier 1973), insects need to rely on olfactory senses in natural habitats in order to reduce the cost of host plant searching. An insect approaches its host plant for feeding, oviposition, or shelter initially, most likely through perception of volatile chemicals. The antenna is the primary olfactory organ of insects, while the maxillary palp is a close range olfactory sensor in food

Journal of Insect Science | www.insectscience.org

Bora et al. selection, as has both olfactory and gustatory sensilla. In lepidopteran larvae, the antennae and maxillary palpi are known to contain about 80% of all the chemoreceptor cells (De Boer 2006). Muga silkworm, Antheraea assamensis Helfer (Lepidoptera: Saturniidae), the producer of golden silk, is a lepidopteran insect endemic to northeastern India. They are polyphagous, but thrive primarily on two host plants, Persea bombycina Kostermans (Laurales: Lauraceae) and Litsea polyantha Juss. Restrictive feeding on a few plants might be the reason for its confinement to northeastern India only. Very few studies have been carried out so far regarding the feeding behavior of A. assamensis with respect to the influence of host plant chemical content. While Hazarika et al. (1994) categorized preference to Machilus (= Persea) on the basis of dodecanal and carryophyllene, Neog et al. (2011) showed a mixture of caryophyllene, decyl aldehyde, and dodecylaldehyde to be attractive for biting behavior of A. assamensis larvae. However, no work has been carried out to probe into the chemosensory basis of the restricted dietbreadth in A. assamensis. In this work, we report the role of the antenna and maxillary palp in food selection by A. assamensis larvae based on food and odor choice tests. Materials and Methods Insects Larvae of A. assamensis that hatched from disease-free eggs obtained from the Government Sericulture Farm were cultured on leaves of its primary host plant, P. bombycina, grown in the botanical garden within the campus of the Department of Life Sciences, Dibrugarh University, Assam, India. Diseasefree eggs were determined by examining females during oviposition for the presence of

             

       2

Journal of Insect Science: Vol. 13 | Article 52 pebrine spores, and eggs produced by females that were free of pebrine spores were considered to be disease-free. After the third moult, larvae were allowed to acclimatize to laboratory conditions (22–30° C) and the early fifth instar larvae were subjected to a food choice test using the method of De Boer and Hanson (1984) with necessary modifications and an odor choice test using a Y-tube olfactometer. The modifications were as follows: (1) in the test container, bamboo sticks were used for holding the fiber disks in place of a pin, and (2) the percent of larvae making a choice was calculated instead of choice index. Plants For carrying out food choice, two host plants, P. bombycina and L. polyantha, and two nonhosts, Litsea grandifolia Teschner (Laurales: Lauraceae) and Ziziphus jujuba Miller (Rosales: Rhamnaceae), were selected. Plant extract preparation Leaf extract was prepared by grinding 100 g of fresh leaves with 100 mL of distilled water in an electric grinder, and then filtering the mixture using double layered muslin cloth. Only freshly prepared filtrate was used in each bioassay. Ablation of the sensory organ Larvae were immobilized on ice for 15-30 minutes and the peripheral sense organs, namely the antenna, maxillary palp, maxillary galea, labrum epipharynx, and labial palp, were removed selectively by microsurgery, keeping only the organ considered for study. Extirpations were performed on the two-dayold fifth instar larvae under a dissecting binocular microscope (Olympus, www.olympus.co.uk). After recovery, the larvae were allowed to feed normally on leaves of their primary host plant. Insects having unsuccessful operations were not considered

Journal of Insect Science | www.insectscience.org

Bora et al. further. Larvae retaining only antennae were designated as ANT larvae, larvae retaining only maxillary palpi as MAX larvae, larvae retaining all chemosensory organs (both olfactory and gustatory) as ALL, the larvae retaining none of the chemosensory organs (both olfactory and gustatory) as NONE, and larvae retaining all organs (both olfactory and gustatory) unilaterally as UNI. Y-Tube olfactometer The behavioral responses to plant extracts prepared from the host and non-host plants were assessed by using one all-glass Y-tube olfactometer (3 cm diameter and 15 cm long) (modified from Blackmer et al. 2004) (Figure 1A). Inside the tube, a glass wire of 0.3 cm thickness was attached in order to help the test insect to move towards the ends of the arms. One end of the stem of the Y-tube was connected to a vacuum pump (KNF, www.knf.com) to suck the air. The ends of the two arms of the Y-tube were connected to two adaptors made of Z-glass, and in each adaptor was placed a piece of Whatman filter paper (1 cm2). Each adaptor was connected to the Rotameter (Sigma-Aldrich, www.sigmaaldrich.com) in order to maintain a constant air-flow. The humidified air was first passed through charcoal filters and then through the rotameter into the adaptors attached to the arms of the Y- tube. Bioassays Food choice test. Bioassay was done through a food choice test carried out in two ways. The two plants food choice test (dual choice test) was carried out between a host plant, P. bombycina or L. polyantha and a non-host, L. grandifolia or Z. jujuba. The one plant food choice test (single choice test) was carried out between a host or non-host plant and water in order to evaluate the degree of preference for different food plants by comparing the plants

             

       3

Journal of Insect Science: Vol. 13 | Article 52 with a neutral medium. Water was used as the neutral medium, as it was used in the preparation of the leaf extract. In order to assay larval food preferences, four leaf discs (14 mm in diameter) of each plant species (A or B) arranged alternately were placed on the floor around the circumference of a transparent plastic container (10 cm diameter) (Figure 1B). Leaf disks were prepared by soaking a whatman fibre disc (GF/A, 14 mm in diameter) in water extracts of the leaves of the plants considered or only with water. The leaf discs were fitted to the distal end of bamboo sticks, whose proximal ends were fixed on hard cardboard kept at 1 cm above the bottom of the container. The bamboo sticks were used to hold the leaf disc like a stem of a plant and to provide crawling space for the larvae. All the larvae were not fed for 2–4 hours before being subjected to the food choice test, and then were placed in the center of the floor of the container. When the larva had eaten about 50% of the area of one of the two plant species (A or B), the test was stopped. The amount of time it took, called T50, varied from 2 minutes to 1 hour from the start of the test. Tests were repeated with a minimum of 10 larvae. The 50% food consumption in T50 time was expressed in terms of percent consumption per minute using the unitary method of mathematical calculation. The percentage of choosing larvae was based on the number of larvae in one group opting for a particular food. Odor choice test. In each assay testing orientation based on odor perception, a fifth instar larva belonging to any of the ALL, UNI, MAX, ANT, and NONE larval groups was released through a 2 cm opening located at the base of the stem of the Y-tube . Airflow was maintained in the Y-tube at 100 mL-1 from the opposite direction with the help of a rotameter

Journal of Insect Science | www.insectscience.org

Bora et al. and a vacuum pump. The freshly prepared extracts of leaves were applied to 1 × 1 cm pieces (Whatman No. 1) of filter paper. The filter paper disk containing the extract of the host plant was kept in one chamber (adapter), and the disk containing the extract of the nonhost plant was kept in the other chamber, so that the insect could perceive the odor moving along the airflow. The amount of time from the moment the insect started walking upwind until it reached 2 cm beyond the Y- junction of the Y-tube was recorded. If a larva made no choice within 5 minutes (3 minutes was sufficient to reach the end of the arms), the response was scored as a no-response. The experiments consisted of a minimum of 10 choices, and the insects showing no response were discarded. After each test, the entire setup was turned 180° to avoid any positional effects. Between each experiment, all parts of the set-up were washed and dried in an oven at 150° C. for 30 minutes. Statistical analysis Preference based on mean percent consumption per minute was analyzed by one (p < 0.025) and two (p < 0.05) tailed MannWhitney tests. A chi-square test was performed between the percent of choosing larvae to show orientation preference. A nonparametric binomial test was used to determine significant differences between the numbers of larvae choosing different plants through odor perception. All analysis was done by SPSS 17. Results Food Choice Test P. bombycina vs. L. grandifolia. Larvae were given a choice between host, P. bombycina, and non-host, L. grandifolia. ALL, UNI, ANT, and MAX larvae opted for P. bombycina (p < 0.025). The mean percent

             

       4

Journal of Insect Science: Vol. 13 | Article 52 consumption per minute was significantly lower in ANT and MAX in comparison to that of UNI larvae (Figure 2C, D). The percent of larvae opting for P. bombycina was 100 in all larvae except NONE larvae, which failed to differentiate between P. bombycina and L. grandifolia (p < 0.05). 40% of NONE larvae opted for P. bombycina, while 60% opted for L. grandifolia. The mean percent consumption per minute was higher in the case of non-hosts (p < 0.05). In the study for evaluating the preference towards the second primary host plant, L. polyantha, larvae were given a choice between L .polyantha and L. grandifolia. ALL, UNI, ANT, and MAX larvae opted for L polyantha (p < 0.025). Mean percent consumption per minute was reduced in MAX and ANT larvae (Figure 2C, D). NONE larvae failed to differentiate between the two choices (p < 0.05), and the percentages of larvae opting for both the plants were equal (50%) (Figure 2E). In NONE larvae, mean percent consumption per minute was higher in the case of non-hosts (p < 0.05). P. bombycina or L. polyantha vs. Z. jujuba. Larvae were given a choice between P. bombycina and Z. jujuba. ALL, UNI, ANT, and MAX larvae opted for P. bombycina, and the percent of larvae opting for P. bombycina was 100 in each case (p < 0.001) (Figure 2). Mean percent consumption of P. bombycina per minute by ANT and MAX larvae was reduced in comparison to that of UNI larvae (Figure 2B– D). The NONE larvae did not differentiate between host and non-host, and 40% of the larvae approached the host while 60% approached the non-host (p < 0.05) (Figure 2E). In NONE larvae, the mean percent consumption per minute was higher in the case of nonhosts (p < 0.05).

Journal of Insect Science | www.insectscience.org

Bora et al. When given a choice between L. polyantha and Z. jujuba, ALL, UNI, ANT, and MAX larvae opted for L. polyantha, and the percent of larvae opting for L. polyantha was 100 in each case (p < 0.001). Mean food consumption per minute was minimum for MAX larvae (Figure 2C). NONE larvae did not differentiate between the host and non-host (Figure 2E). The percent of larvae opting for both the plants was equal, but the mean percent consumption per minute was higher in case of the non-host (p < 0.05). P. bombycina or L. polyantha vs. water. When larvae were given a choice between P. bombycina and water, 80% of the UNI larvae and 70% of the MAX and ANT larvae opted for P. bombycina. Only 20% of the NONE larvae opted for P. bombycina, and the remaining 80% opted for water (p < 0.001). The mean percent consumption per minute was higher in UNI and ALL larvae in the case of consumption of host plant against the consumption of water (p < 0.05). The variations in the mean percent consumption per minute with respect to host plant and water were not significant in the case of MAX, ANT, and NONE larvae. When larvae were given a choice between L. polyantha and water, ALL and UNI larvae opted for only L. polyantha, and the mean percent consumption per minute was highly significant (p < 0.025). 70% of MAX and 20% of ANT larvae opted for L. polyantha, and the rest opted for water. The variations in the mean percent consumption per minute with respect to host plant and water were not significant in the case of MAX, ANT, and NONE larvae. Similar to ANT larvae, only 20% of NONE larvae opted for L. polyantha (Figure 3).

             

       5

Journal of Insect Science: Vol. 13 | Article 52 L. grandifolia vs. water. Larvae were given a choice between L. grandifolia and water. ALL, UNI, ANT, and MAX larvae opted for only water, and in each case none of the larvae opted for the non-host (p < 0.001). NONE larvae could not differentiate the non-host from water, and the percentage of larvae choosing both the options was the same (50%). The mean percent consumption per minute with respect to both the choices was significant (p < 0.05) (Figure 3). Z. jujuba vs. water. Larvae were given a choice between Z. jujuba and water. ALL, UNI, ANT, and MAX larvae preferred only water over Z. jujuba (p < 0.001), and mean percent consumption was highly significant (p < 0.025). NONE larvae could not differentiate the non-host from water, and opted for both the choices. The percent of larvae choosing the non-host was 40, and the percent choosing water was 60 (p < 0.05) (Figure 3). The variation in mean percent consumption between the two choices was significant (p < 0.05). Odor choice test When ablated larvae were given a choice between the odor of a host versus non-hosts, ALL, UNI, ANT, and MAX larvae opted for only the host plant odor (p < 0.001) and only NONE larvae opted for both the choices (p = 1.0). When the choice was given between the odor of a host plant versus water in the case of P. bombycina, 80% of ALL, UNI, ANT, and MAX larvae opted for the odor of P. bombycina (p < 0.01) and 80% of NONE larvae opted for water (p < 0.05). In the case of L. polyantha, 100% of ALL and UNI larvae (p < 0.001), 60% of MAX larvae (p < 0.05), and 20% of ANT (p < 0.05) and NONE larvae opted for L. polyantha (p < 0.05). In the case of the non-hosts, L. grandifolia and Z. jujuba, all the larvae opted for water except the

Journal of Insect Science | www.insectscience.org

Bora et al. NONE larvae, which opted for both the choices (p < 0.001) (Figure 4). Discussion Larvae of A. assamensis feed on a very narrow range of host plants. Before the time of hatching, straw sticks containing the eggs of A. assamensis are tied to the tree trunk base. The newly hatched larvae, after taking several bites of the eggshells, crawl upwards in search of leaves, where they continue feeding until the leaf stock becomes exhausted. Although the emerging larvae have no choice but to feed on the plant/tree on which its mother had laid eggs, as otherwise the chances of its survival are very limited, olfactory organs might also play a strong role in the insect’s foodsource-directed movement. The blend of volatile chemicals released by host plants either as constitutive or induced defense (Turling et al. 1995; Agarwal 1998; Walling 2000; Schoohoven et al. 2005) are perceived by olfactory organs. In order to understand the contribution of olfactory receptors in food preference behavior of A. assamensis larvae, ablation of all other chemosensory organs, leaving either the maxillary palp or antenna only, was done before subjecting the larvae to food and odor choice tests. As all the chemosensory organs are bilaterally represented, ablation of all chemosensory organs of one side should nullify the probable effect of surgery on food preference. Therefore, all tests of significance were carried out taking UNI larvae as the control group. In the test related to the preference of a host plant over the non-host L. grandifolia, ALL, and UNI larvae showed preference for the host plant in the dual choice test. Therefore, each of the two peripheral organs were competent alone for mediating host preference, but none was absolutely necessary for the media-

             

       6

Journal of Insect Science: Vol. 13 | Article 52 tion. Similarly, each of them were also competent in rejecting the non-host, as the NONE larvae failed to differentiate between host and non-host. In Manduca sexta, while antennae were recorded to mediate acceptance and rejection behavior for plants tested, maxillary palp mediated preference for normally rejected plant species (De Boer 2006). When preference for a host plant was studied by using one plant choice test, 70–100% of both ANT and MAX larvae preferred P. bombycina over water. In the absence of the antenna and maxillary palp, 80% of the NONE larvae preferred water over the host plants (Figure 3). This result confirmed that the larvae required either the antenna or the maxillary palp for mediating normal food preference. Many studies on the physiological basis of such oligophagy carried out in M. sexta larvae, a facultative specialist on solanaceous plants, have led to the conclusion that discrimination of food choice mostly depends on the chemical content of the food (De Boer et al. 1992), and a decision is made on a preconstruction recognition template, be it innate or acquired (Del Campo et al. 2001). The chemicals present in plants may also modulate functional significance of the chemosensory organs in particular and food choice behavior of the insect as a whole (Glendinning et al. 2000; Chapman 2003). For instance, when tuned to host-specialized chemical recognition cues, M. sexta remained oligophagous, but was otherwise polyphagous (Del Campo and Miles 2003). When the choice was given between the host, L. polyantha, and water, only ablated larvae either with an antenna or a maxillary palp opted for both the choices. ALL and UNI larvae opted only for the host. While 70% of MAX larvae opted for both the hosts, P. bombycina and L. polyantha, in the single plant choice test for each plant, 70% of ANT larvae opted

Journal of Insect Science | www.insectscience.org

Bora et al. for P. bombycina in the choice test between P. bombycina and water. Only 20% ANT and NONE larvae opted for L. polyantha in the choice test between L. polyantha and water. This result might be due to the prior feeding history of the larvae. As all the larvae were grown on leaves of P. bombycina prior to the tests, antennae may have become tuned to the odor of P. bombycina only. Hence, when the larvae were exposed to the odor of L. polyantha, they could not recognize L. polyantha on first exposure as their host. Induction of feeding preference by diet was earlier reported in M. sexta (De Boer 1992). As per our studies using SEM, the maxillary palp of A. assamensis contains eight sensilla basiconica in a groove and, similar to other Lepidopterans (Dethier and Crnjar 1982), three out of them located centrally,with a unique grooved structure are olfactory, and five located peripherally, each with a small terminal pore, are gustatory (unpublished data). The antennae of A. assamensis contain sensilla styloconica, sensilla basiconica, and basiconic pegs in the pedicel region (Hazarika and Bordoloi 1998). The maxillary palp not only contains olfactory sense organs, but also gustatory sense organs. Therefore, after the initial approach to the host plant based on olfactory perception, the taste receptors probably played a somewhat dominant role in the feedingacceptance decision-making process, and hence the MAX larvae exhibited equal preference towards both the host plants. Heisswolf et al. (2007) showed the stronger preference of a monophagous Chrysomelid beetle for its host plant to be based on contact cues rather than on odor cues. Similar to L. grandifolia, in the food choice test for the non-host, Z. jujuba, the antennae and maxillary palp were individually competent in the rejection of the non-host, and in their absence the NONE larvae could not dif-

             

       7

Journal of Insect Science: Vol. 13 | Article 52 ferentiate the non-host from the host and water. For the NONE larvae, the mean consumption per minute was higher during the consumption of the non-host and water disk. Thus, in the case of both the non-host plants, rejection behavior was regulated by the antennae and the maxillary palp. However, the mean consumption per minute in MAX and ANT larvae in many of the choice tests conducted were reduced in comparison to ALL and UNI larvae. This result might indicate that a complement of organs may be involved in food-acceptance decisions and biting and chewing activities (Boer and Hanson 1975) because only ALL and UNI larvae contained the full complement of the sensory organs involved in olfaction and gustation. The receptor neurons in other gustatory sensilla might contribute to lowering or enhancing the total excitatory input provided by odorant molecules. Biting activity is a temporal activity governed by the central nervous system. A central pattern generator for chewing located in the suboesophageal ganglion has been shown to be inhibited by thoracic input (Griss et al. 1991; Rowel and Simpson 1992; Rohrbacher 1994). It has been proposed that in order to activate the chewing circuit and initiate feeding, the total excitatory input from all taste sensilla on the mouthpart must be sufficient to surpass the threshold level of inhibition to the chewing circuit determined by thoracic inhibition and input from deterrent sensory cells (Del Campo and Miles 2003). Like other insects (Saxena and Schoonhoven 1978, 1982; Asaoka and Akai 1991; Mondy et al. 1998; Asaoka 2000; Schoonhooven and Loon 2002; William III et al. 2010), odor cues emanated by the host plants may play an important role in host plant selection by A. assamensis larvae, owing to the participation of olfactory organs. In addition to the ingestive tests, when the ablated larvae were

Journal of Insect Science | www.insectscience.org

Bora et al. subjected to odor choice test using the Y-tube olfactometer, all larvae except the NONE larvae were attracted to only the host plant odor in dual plant choice tests. In conformity with the ingestive test, when the odor test was performed for host versus water, the percentage of MAX and ANT larvae opting for P. bombycina was equal to that of the control. However, the percentage of ANT and MAX larvae opting for L. polyantha was significantly reduced compared to that of the control. Except for in the case of L. polyantha, ANT larvae behaved similarly to NONE larvae. The results reconfirmed the tuning of the antennal receptor to host plant odor based on prior feeding history. Thus, in wild habitat, host plant chemicals play key roles in orientation and food selection through the participation of the antenna and maxillary palp in A. assamensis. Acknowledgments The authors are grateful to the Department of Science and Technology, New Delhi, India, and UGC, India, for a financial grant. The authors are also grateful to the three anonymous reviewers for their valuable comments for improving the quality of the work. References Agarwal AA. 1998. Induced responses to herbivory and increased plant performance. Science 279: 1201–1202. Asaoka K, Akai H. 1991. Morphology of the larval taste organs of Antheraea yamamai and their spike responses to stimulation with certain sugars and alkaloids. In: Akai H, Kiuchi M, Editors. Wild silk moths. pp. 35-42. The International Society for Wild Silkmoths.

             

       8

Journal of Insect Science: Vol. 13 | Article 52

Bora et al.

Asaoka K, Shibuya T. 1995. Morphological and electrophysiological characteristics of the epipharyngeal sensilla of the silkworm, Bombyx mori. Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata 77: 167–176.

De Boer G, Hanson FE. 1987. Food plant selection and induction of feeding preference among host and nonhost plants in larvae of tobacco hornworm, Manduca sexta. Physiological Entomology 12: 387–398.

Asaoka K. 2000. Deficiency of gustatory sensitivity to some deterrent compounds in polyphagous mutant strains of the silkworm, Bombyx mori. Journal of Comparative Physiology 186: 1011–1018.

De Boer G, Schmitt AS, Zavod R, Mitscher LA. 1992. Feeding stimulatory and inhibitory chemicals from an acceptable nonhost plant for Manduca sexta: improved detection by larvae deprived of selected chemosensory organs. Journal of Chemical Ecology 18: 885– 895.

Blackmer JL, Rodriguez-Saona C, Byers JA, Shope KL, Smith JP. 2004. Behavioral response of Lygus hesperus to conspecifics and headspace volatiles of alfalfa in a Y-tube olfactometer. Journal of Chemical Ecology 30: 1547–1564. Bullas-Appleton ES, Otis G, Gillard C, Schaafsma AW. 2004. Potato leafhopper (Homoptera: Cicadellidae) varietal preferences in edible beans in relation to visual and olfactory cue. Environmental Entomology 33: 1381–1388. Chapman RF. 2003. Contact chemoreception in feeding by phytophagous insects. Chapman and Hall. De Boer G, Hanson FE. 1975. Differentiation of roles of chemosensory organs in food discrimination among host and non-host plants by larvae of the tobacco hornworm, Manduca sexta. Physiological Entomology 12: 187–198. De Boer G, Hanson FE. 1984. Foodplant selection and induction of feeding preference among host and non-host plants in larvae of the tobacco hornworm Manduca sexta. Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata 35: 177– 193.

De Boer G. 1992. Diet induced food preference by Manduca sexta larvae: acceptable non host plants elicit a stronger induction than host plants. Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata 63:3–12. De Boer G. 2006. The role of antennae and maxillary palps in mediating food preference by larvae of the tobacco hornworm, Manduca sexta. Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata 119: 29–38. Del Campo ML, Miles CI, Schroeder FC, Muller C, Booker R, Renwick JAA. 2001. Host recognition by the tobacco hornworm is mediated by host plant compound. Nature 411: 186–189. Del Campo ML, Miles CI. 2003. Chemosensory tuning to host recognition cues in the facultative specialist larvae of the moth Manduca sexta. Journal of Experimental Biology 206: 3979–3990. Dethier VG, Crnjar RM. 1982. Candidate code in the gustatory system of caterpillars. Journal of General Physiology 79:543–569. Glendinning JI, Nelson NM, Bernays EA. 2000. How do inositol and glucose modulate

Journal of Insect Science | www.insectscience.org

             

       9

Journal of Insect Science: Vol. 13 | Article 52 feeding in Manduca sexta caterpillars? Journal of Experimental Biology 203: 1299–1315. Griss C, Simpson JR, Rowel CHF. 1991. Localization in the central nervous system of larval Manduca sexta (Lepidoptera: Sphingidae) of areas responsible for aspects of feeding behavior. Journal of Insect Physiology 37: 477–482. Hanson FE, Dethier VG. 1973. Role of gestation and olfaction in food plant discrimination in the tobacco hornworm, Manduca sexta. Journal of Insect Physiology 19: 1019–1034. Hazarika LK, Bordoloi S. 1998. Antennal and mouth part sensilla of the muga silk worm A. assama (Lepidoptera: Saturniidae). Sericologia 38: 55–63. Hazarika RL, Baruah JN, Deka PC, Hazarika AK, Kataky JCS. 1994. Chemical investigation of the essential oil of Machilus bombycina King. Indian Perfumer 36(1): 133–136. Heisswolf A, Gabler D, Obermaier E, Caroline M. 2007. Olfactory versus contact cues in host plant recognition of a monophagous Chrysomelid beetle. Journal of Insect Behavior 20: 247–266. Jorgensen K, Almas TJ, Marion-Poll F, Mustapatra H. 2007. Electrophysiological characterization of responses from gustatory receptor neurons of sensilla chaetica in the moth Heliothis virescens. Chemical Senses. 32: 863–879. Mitchell BK, Itagaki H, Rivet MP. 1999. Peripheral and central structures involved in insect gestation. Microscopy research and technique 47: 401–415.

Journal of Insect Science | www.insectscience.org

Bora et al. Mondy N, Pracros P, Fermaud M, CorioCostet MF. 1998. Olfactory and gustatory behaviour by larvae of Lobesia botrana in response to Botrytis cinerea. Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata 88: 1–7. Neog K, Unni B, Ahmed G. 2011. Studies on the influence of host plants and effect of chemical stimulants on the feeding behavior in the muga silkworm, Antheraea assamensis. Journal of Insect Science 11:133. Available online: http://www.insectscience.org/11.133/ Reisenman CE, Christensen TA, Hilderbrand JG. 2005. Chemosensory selectivity of output neurons innervating an identified, sexually isomorphic olfactory glomerulus. Journal of Neuroscience. 25: 8017–8026. Riffel JA, Lei H, Hildebrand JG. 2009. Neural correlates of behavior in moth Manduca sexta in response to complex odors. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 106: 19219–19226. Rohrbacher J. 1994. Fictive chewing activity in motor neurons and interneurons of the suboesophageal ganglion of Manduca sexta larvae. Journal of Comparative Physiology 175: 629–637. Rowel CHF, Simpson SJ. 1992. A peripheral input of thoracic origin inhibits chewing movements in the larvae of Manduca sexta. Journal of Insect Physiology. 38: 475–483. Saxena KN, Schoonhoven LM. 1978. Induction of orientational and feeding preferences in Manduca sexta larvae for an artificial diet containing citral. Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata 23: 72–78. Saxena KN, Schoonhoven LM. 1982. Induction of orientation and feeding preferences in

             

       10

Journal of Insect Science: Vol. 13 | Article 52 Manduca sexta larvae for different food sources. Entomologia Experimentalis et applicata 32: 173–180. Schoonhoven LM, Dethier VG. 1966. Sensory aspects of host plant discrimination by lepidopterous larvae. Archieves Neerlandaises de Zoologie 16: 497–530. Schoonhoven LM, Loon JJA, Dicke M. 2005. Insect-Plant Biology, second edition. Oxford University press. Schoonhoven LM, van Loon JJA. 2002. An inventory of taste in caterpillars: Each species its own key. Acta Zoologica Academiae scientiarum hungaricae 48: 215–263. Schoonhoven LM. 1987. What makes a caterpillar eat? The sensory codes underlying feeding behaviour. In: Chapman RF, Bernays EA, Stoffolano JG, Editors. Advances in chemoreception and behaviour. pp. 69–97. Springer. Shiraiwa T. 2008. Multimodal Chemosensory Integration through the Maxillary Palp in Drosophila. PLoS ONE 3(5): e2191. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0002191.

Journal of Insect Science | www.insectscience.org

Bora et al. Stadler E, Hanson F. 1975. Olfactory capabilities of the gustatory chemoreceptors of the tobacco hornworm larvae. Journal of Comparative Physiology. 104: 97–102. Steinbrecht RA. 1997. Pore structures in insect olfactory sensilla: a review of data and concepts. International Journal of Insect Morphology And Embryology 26: 229–245. Ting A, Ma X, Hanson FE. 2002. Induction of feeding preference in larvae of patch butterfly, Chlosyne lacinia. Acta Zoologica Academiae Scientiarum Hungaricae. 48: 281–295. Turlings TCJ, Loughrin JH, McCall PJ, Rose USR, Lewis WJ, Tumlinson JH. 1995. How caterpillar-damaged plants protect themselves by attracting parasitic wasp. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 92: 4169–4174. Walling LL. 2000. The myriad plant responses to herbivore. Journal of Plant Growth Regulations 19: 195–216. Williams L, Blackmer JL, Rodriguez-Saona C, Zhu S. 2010. Plant Volatiles Influence Electrophysiological and Behavioral Responses of Lygus Hesperus. Journal of Chemical Ecology 36: 467–478.

             

       11

Journal of Insect Science: Vol. 13 | Article 52

Bora et al.

Figure 1. A: Scheme of Y-tube olfactometer. B: Scheme of leaf disk arrangement for food choice tests. High quality figures are available online.

Figure 2. Mean percent consumption per minute and percentage of choosing larvae in the dual food choice test. High quality figures are available online.

Journal of Insect Science | www.insectscience.org

             

       12

Journal of Insect Science: Vol. 13 | Article 52

Bora et al.

Figure 3. Mean percent consumption per minute and percentage of choosing larvae in the single plant food choice test. High quality figures are available online.

Figure 4. Percentage of larvae opting for odor choice in Y-tube olfactometer. High quality figures are available online.

Journal of Insect Science | www.insectscience.org

             

       13