mixedfeelings - UTS ePRESS

19 downloads 0 Views 102KB Size Report
places, including the present, as Anand Pandian recently ... how to express or suppress those feelings? ... Anand Pandian, comments at the final panel of the.
This collection of essays aims to introduce students of anthropology to that discipline’s contribution to the interdisciplinary field of work on the emotions. It should be said at the outset that it is reviewed here by a nonanthropologist who is unable to assess whether it adequately represents the history of vicis-

ANNA GIBBS

situdes of thought about emotion in anthropology, or the range of current anthropological

mixed feelings

thinking on a topic which has emerged as a pervasive concern across the humanities and social sciences over the last decade. Having said that, as a reader from another discipline I found that the introductory essay by Maruska Svasek gives a broad but useful overview of the history of the main currents of thought about emotion

K AY M I LT O N A N D M A R U S K A S VA S E K ( E D S )

in anthropology, which she characterises as on

Mixed Emotions: Anthropological Studies of Feeling

the one hand a European, broadly psycho-

Berg, Oxford and New York, 2005 ISBN RRP

analytic concern with kinship in which culture is generated by ‘drives’ (e.g. Malinowski),

9781845200794

and on the other, the US-based ‘culture and

15.95 (pb)

personality’ mode with its strong interest in developmental psychology (Benedict, Mead). She delineates the differences between anthropological, psychological and sociological approaches to the topic, and reads current moves in anthropology as a shift from an emphasis on discourse to one on embodiment. This in itself entails an opening to disciplines beyond the social sciences, and although the introduction does not fully address this, it soon becomes clear in what follows that anthropology is beginning to do so, albeit in a way that strikes this reader as somewhat erratic. The opening chapter by Kay Milton elaborates on analytic effects across the disciplines

226

VOLUME13 NUMBER2 SEP2007

of the form taken by the western distinction reflects on the way in which her own emphasis between nature and culture, and proposes an on pragmatism and political strategy conceals ‘ecological’ approach similar to that taken to the strength of felt resentment as a motivating perception by J.J. Gibson as a way out of the force in Kewa social life, and as a significant opposition between them.1 This would mean shaper of Kewa selfhood. Describing resentment paying attention to the specificity of an organ- as ‘weak person’s witchcraft’, she shows that fear ism’s connection to its environment, which in of provoking it provides an important check on turn constrains what can be learned from that expressions of contempt and disdain, which environment. Milton draws on the psychologi- would be met with an immediate, violent cal work of Neisser (on memory) and Izard, on response. Critical of the cognitivism of Solomon Damasio in neuroscience and Scherer in sociol- and others who view emotions as interogy to insist on the importance of emotion to pretations, Josephides draws on Nussbaum, learning, and on the importance of learning in Kant, Heidegger and others to argue that interpreting the ‘same’ physiological responses emotions are motivating forces, expressive of in different contexts. For example, a quicken- internal states, and, in Lutz and Abu-Lughod’s ing heartbeat and tightening stomach muscles formulation, they are ‘pragmatic acts and commay indicate fear, anxiety or love, depending municative performances’ that exceed dison whether one is contemplating a snake, an course.2 But it is not clear to me whether she is exam, or a new lover. Milton implies, without contending that emotions are simply likely to actually saying so, that the process of such indi- give rise to certain kinds of action, or that they vidual affective socialisation may then be are already actions in themselves. What is generalised so that we may imagine the pos- clearer in her discussion is that emotions prosibilities of different forms of socialisation in duce the self as both interiority capable of selfdifferent cultures. Although I think she is reflection and agent seeking recognition in absolutely right about the need for this kind of the world. approach, I found it frustrating that I came

Josephides describes the peculiar character

away with no concrete sense of what this might of Kewa resentment as anger with a very be like in an anthropological study.

specific source: an insult to the self. Any reader

On the other hand, Lisette Josephides’s essay familiar with the work of American psychollater in the volume, on ‘Resentment as a Sense ogist Silvan S. Tomkins may wonder why she of Self’, does give more of a picture of the pos- doesn’t simply call it an angry response to the sibilities here, though without actually claiming experience of shame, for this is just what she her work as ecological. Revisiting old field notes seems to be describing. Here anger, rather than and realising that her subsequent writing did no the complementary response of contempt, or justice to the intensity of emotions she recalled the equally conceivable response of distress, is in the social life she describes, Josephides the culturally mandated response to potential

A N N A G I B B S —MIXED FEELINGS

227

humiliation and it is accompanied by certain actually makes Tomkins’s work—and its familiar behavioural repertoires whose pre- absence from any consideration at all in this dictability is surely central to their effectiveness volume—all the more remarkable, since he was as threat.

a psychologist who used psychology against

In fact, I find myself often exasperated by the grain. Moreover, he carried out extensive the fact that these essays often seem to retrace research (but not, as far as I know, actual fieldarguments that Tomkins’s work has simply work) into affective expression and socialisoutflanked—and then I wonder whether I am ation in China and India. Though this work not simply guilty of an over-investment in it was never written up for publication, Tomkins’s which may blind me to the questions it fails to notes are available (unsorted) in the History ask. Nevertheless, there is unquestionably a of Psychology Archives at the University of strange silence around the name of Tomkins in Akron, Ohio. this volume, most obviously in Peter J. Bowler’s

My feelings of impatience with much of the

essay, ‘Darwin on the Expression of the theoretical argument (though not the ethnoEmotions’. Arguing that Darwin’s evolutionary graphy) in these essays derives from the fact approach to the affects failed to be taken up that, for all that talk of affect seems to be omniuntil Paul Ekman did so in the 1970s, which present these days, the development of affect provided an enabling context for renewed theory seems to have been forestalled by a interest in the biologically-informed study of certain defensiveness about the project in the emotional expression, Bowler does not say that face of local resistance in the social sciences, Ekman was in fact one of Tomkins’s graduate which have tended to privilege particular forms students and that his work is deeply indebted of rationality and to regard any approach to to Tomkins’s in many respects, including his thinking affect as marking a dangerous decline theory of innate discrete affects. Bowler does into subjectivism. The social sciences in parshow in some detail that the reasons Ekman ticular seem to have been drawn down the offers for the contemporary resistance to dead end of a debate about whether emotions Darwin are anachronistic, and that it was the are natural or cultural phenomena (often withideology of evolutionary progress that posed out recognising that the distinction itself is culthe major obstacle to the development of turally produced), between a universalising, Darwinian thought on the affects. By the time essentialising biologism on the one hand and a Freud interests himself in Darwin, progression- newer orthodoxy of cultural constructivism on ism was being more widely challenged, and the other. Critiques of the former have become anthropology and sociology ‘refused to privi- second nature. The latter was extensively lege European society as the goal to which all critiqued as so ingrained as to be reflexive by others were moving’ (51), even if in psychology Eve Kosofsky Sedgwick and Adam Frank in behaviourism expelled biology altogether. This their introduction to Shame and its Sisters, the

228

VOLUME13 NUMBER2 SEP2007

volume of writings that introduced the work of adjustment, which again makes possible Tomkins (marginal to psychology, although it exchange and dialogue. Unless, that is, the had been actively taken up by some contem- shame involved is too intense, and humiliation porary psychotherapies) into cultural studies generates rage: repeated sequences of shame in 1995.3 Sedgwick and Frank proposed that and rage (the ‘shame-rage spiral’, as it has been it may be more productive to look beyond called) fragments the self and makes thought familiar theoretical routines to what has now impossible. This suggests the need for modulabecome a rapidly burgeoning interdisciplinary tion of the shame we may inadvertently profield in which biology in particular no longer duce in others, and the importance of trying to functions as the sign of essentialism, but as a think in the face of shame, to think with shame, potential opening onto other ways of thinking rather than attempting to avoid it altogether. relations (including relations between terms Shame signals something in the dialogue that and relations) drawn from nonlinear dynamics requires attention. Anthropological fieldwork and systems and complexity theories. (If this inevitably generates shame in the distinction sounds a little like the structuralism of the it instantiates between subject and object, 1960s, then yes, everything old is new again, as researcher and researched—a shame which it so often is in the history of thought.) This ‘long-term participant observation’ can never coincides with a renewed interest in thinking completely abolish, and which may indeed both cognition and memory as distributed generate its own further sources of shame, as systems, neither fully localisable in the brain Jennifer Biddle has shown in writing about the (neurons, for example, also reside in the heart, sudden trauma of a car accident and the death the gut, the knee, and so on) nor independent that followed it, which shattered the routine of the sensory and affective systems which are and ongoing traumatisation of such an immerindispensable to them. Such a conception of sive experience as intensive fieldwork in a cognition implies certain things about thought, remote community.5 So too she amply demonincluding its partiality (in both senses of the strates it in the writing of her story ‘Yarla’, word) and its dependence on figuration.

a trenchant fable about the responsibilities both

‘The essential dynamic of unification in enforced and assumed by relations established theory construction, in science and in affect through fieldwork, which introduces her new theory construction alike, is error and incon- book on Warlpiri women’s art.6 But fieldwork, sistency’, writes Tomkins.4 Such failures, essen- of course, is also the strength of much contemtial though they are to further thought, porary anthropology, since it forces active negonevertheless give rise to shame, to an inter- tiation with one’s own shame and the shame of ruption or attenuation of ongoing interest and others, of oneself as cause as well as site of enjoyment, rather than to its absolute rupture. shame. It requires precisely the kind of negoThis is the pause that allows for reflection and tiation and exchange with the other that is the

A N N A G I B B S —MIXED FEELINGS

229

source of so much misery and misunderstand- of free indirect discourse in which Warlpiri ing when it is refused or avoided out of fear, English and untranslated Warlpiri terms interhabit, or shame itself.

rupt and relativise the smooth taken-for-

The essays on fieldwork in this book dis- grantedness manufactured by certain forms of appoint even as they do usefully inform the scholarly discourse, and in which structures of uninformed about the parameters of the dis- repetition not usual in English (though their cipline. They tend to place the reader alongside use is doubtless inspired by Tomkins’s famous the writer in the position of relative mastery concatenations) move towards elegy at moments afforded by professional knowledge without in her essay on death and trauma or mimic the rendering the crucial experience of not know- amplification of shame by positive feedback in ing, or something of the limits of understand- her essay on that the workings of that affect in ing. What is missing is the rendering of sensory a very particular intercultural context.7 immersion in another culture, the experiences

Like much of Biddle’s writing, Tonkin’s anec-

of dislocation, surprise and tentative discovery dotes testify to the somatically ingrained nature that require the poetic writing of a Lingis or a of affective knowledge, but also make clear that Biddle, or the descriptive and narrative powers affects are cultured. The distinction between of a novelist—perhaps even, at times, a comic affect and emotion seems to allow for a clearer novelist. Elizabeth Tonkin narrates secondhand understanding of the corporeality of affect, an encounter that makes some of the immediate both the way in which affect is inevitably of the difficulties of negotiation clear. A linguist in body, and the way in which this means it feels South India asks a street vendor if he sells real. This is the essence of affect as a (or even cigarettes, and when the vendor slowly shakes the) motivator in human life. Just as affect is an his head the linguist automatically turns away in interface between the self and the social, so too disappointment—even though he ‘knows’ that it is an interface between consciousness and in this cultural context the headshake means what remains unconscious. We are only ever yes. (56) Did the vendor ‘feel puzzled at the partially in control of our own affective exprescustomer’s rejection, or personally criticised in sion: we can communicate without wanting to; some way?’, Tonkin wonders. When she writes we can dissimulate, but only with difficulty conthat ‘imagination makes real’ (58), one might ceal our dissimulation. Even the muscles we can wish for more of it in the writing of these move voluntarily are dependent on the autoessays, though perhaps the fault here lies less nomic neurons which function beyond the conwith the writers than with the strictures of the trol of the neocortex (which we tend to imagine publishing industry which wants textbooks for as ‘higher’ intelligence, the locus of consciousthe largest possible number of large courses. ness in general and the will in particular). These My own feeling is that students are more likely in turn respond to the environment (including to be excited by the way in which Jennifer the social environment) which is itself not fully Biddle’s writing makes inventive use of a kind separable from us but is partially brought into

230

VOLUME13 NUMBER2 SEP2007

being by our movement in it as recent work on learned and cultured as a form of social regulavision by writers such as Francisco Varela or tion. Moreover, the distinction between affect Rudolfo Llinas makes clear. In Merleau-Ponty’s and emotion helps us think with more care succinct summation, ‘behaviour is the first cause about the constitution of subjectivity both hisof all stimulations’.8 Affects are not local but torically and culturally, and the ways in which systemic responses which feed back into the different forms of subjectivity are enfolded with body and which are represented by it to itself the social and cultural milieu in which we and to others. The feedback system extends move, which shapes us and which we in turn beyond the boundaries of the individual body may shape. It allows us to analyse different and is mediated by others and by cultural formations of social responsiveness so that amplifiers such as media. Affects produce emotions can then be seen as the social and culinstant changes in the body’s chemistry and in tural technologies of affect generating a range of hedonic tone, all of which produces new dis- available behavioural repertoires. positions in the world and readiness to certain

It is crucial that the theory of discrete affects

kinds of action (including further affective be brought into dialogue with historical studies response, and reflection) rather than others.

of the emotions that periodise ways of thinking

The same distinction also allows for certain about emotion and with those that focus on the kinds of work to be done: it makes possible social regulation of affect at different times and studies of discrete affects rather than of a places, including the present, as Anand Pandian generalised, encompassing, but essentially de- recently pointed out.9 Kay Milton concludes materialised ‘affect’ in the singular; it gives us a the volume under discussion by articulating precise indication of how and where different three questions to guide further research: how affects inhere in the body; it provides an affect do people in different cultures learn what and dynamics on the basis of the different neuro- how to feel about what; how do they learn to logical and physiological profiles of the dif- perceive specific bodily sensations as particular ferent affects which then enables the tracing of feelings; and how do they learn whether and specific lines of force and their intersection how to express or suppress those feelings? with other such lines, and it opens the way to a These are important questions, and perhaps study of the differential socialisation of the they should have been the ones contributors affects and their (con)scripting in certain cul- were explicitly asked to address, along with tural and social narratives. It also allows us to developing explicitly the ecological approach think what the affective components of complex proposed by Milton. This is obliquely taken and culturally specific emotional formations like up in John Knight’s essay on the creation of amae, song, or ressentiment may be, and to try to ‘emotional affinities’ between monkeys and grasp the immediate contagious and automatic humans in Japanese monkey parks, but an elements in the corporeal activation of these opportunity is missed to theorise the approach formations, as well as that in them which is explicitly here.

A N N A G I B B S —MIXED FEELINGS

231

These, finally, are all interesting and worth- something of its openness to the world that while essays which all contribute to an exam- both shapes and is shaped by it. ination of the ways in which the five emotions

——————————

Ekman and other post-Darwinian writers broadly agree have a very high degree of inter-

ANNA GIBBS

is Associate Professor in the School

cultural intelligibility (fear, anger, sadness, of Communication Arts, University of Western enjoyment and disgust) are differently social- Sydney. She is currently working on an ARC ised in different cultures, and how they are funded project, The Power of the Image: Affect, wrought into complex cultural emotional forms Audiences and Disturbing Imagery, with Virginia and practices which don’t necessarily translate Nightingale, and her most recent essay on affect into each other easily or at all—even if some of theory appears in Deborah Staines (ed), Interthese writers would not accept that view of rogating the War on Terror, Cambridge Scholars affect (as opposed to emotion) as innate. But Publishing, 2007. they do also testify to the ways in which affect

——————————

theory requires a sustained interdisciplinary (as well as disciplinary) endeavour, since the location of affect at the interface of nature and

1. J.J. Gibson, The Ecological Approach to Visual Percep-

tion, Houghton Mifflin, Boston,1979. 2. C.A. Lutz and L. Abu-Lughod (eds), Language and the

culture, self and the social, cognition and the senses, means that it is inevitably constituted

3.

differently as an object by a number of incommensurable disciplinary knowledges. Some of these disciplines, I would suggest, have also

4.

produced problems that might dissolve or

5.

which might appear otherwise if Milton’s ecological approach were to be rigorously and

6.

explicitly developed and it was possible to see what difference such an approach actually

7.

made in practice to the particular studies of 8.

emotion presented here. Such an approach, it seems to me, would challenge any absolute distinction between culture and the bio-physical realm, and would want also to take account of

9.

Politics of Emotion, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 1990, p. 11. Eve Kosofsky Sedgwick and Adam Frank, ‘Shame in the Cybernetic Fold: Reading Silvan Tomkins’, in E.K. Sedgwick and A. Frank (eds), Shame and its Sisters: A Silvan Tomkins Reader, Duke University Press, Durham,1995, pp. 1–28. Silvan S. Tomkins, Affect, Imagery and Consciousness, vol. II, Springer, New York, 1963, p. 463. Jennifer L. Biddle, ‘Bruises That Won’t Heal: Melancholic Identification and other Ethnographic Hauntings’, Mortality, vol. 7, no. 1, 2002, pp. 96–110. Jennifer L. Biddle, ‘Yarla: A Story about a Painting’, in Breasts, Bodies, Canvas, UNSW Press, Sydney, 2007, pp. 7–10. Jennifer L. Biddle, ‘Shame’, Australian Feminist Studies, 12, 1997, pp. 227–40. In Francisco Varela, Evan Thompson and Eleanor Rosch, The Embodied Mind: Cognitive Science and Human Experience, MIT Press, Cambridge Mass., 1993, p. 174. Anand Pandian, comments at the final panel of the ‘Decolonising Affect Theory’ Conference, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, June 2006.

the different ways in which affect interfaces with communications media (television, the internet, dance, ritual, etc.) so as ultimately to call into question the limits of ‘the human’ itself, to highlight its potential plasticity, and

232

VOLUME13 NUMBER2 SEP2007