Parsing Strategies in L1 and L2 Sentence Processing - CiteSeerX

0 downloads 0 Views 238KB Size Report
as a modifier of the first DP (l'avocat) or the second one (la chanteuse): ..... to answer a content question after having read each sentence, e.g. 'Was the trainer ...
The Role of Lexical and Contextual Information in Parsing Ambiguous Sentences in Greek

Despina Papadopoulou (University of Crete)

Harald Clahsen* (University of Essex)

Running head: Parsing Ambiguous Sentences in Greek * Corresponding Author: Harald Clahsen Department of Linguistics University of Essex Colchester C04 3SQ UK email: [email protected] fax: +44-1206-87-2085

Parsing ambiguous sentences in Greek 2

ABSTRACT

This study reports results from a sentence completion task and a self-paced reading experiment examining relative clause (RC) attachment preferences in native speakers of Greek. To determine the role of lexical-semantic as well as referential context information for RC attachment, we examined two constructions under different context conditions: RCs preceded by complex noun phrases with genitives [DP-1+DP-2Gen] and RCs preceded by complex noun phrases containing prepositional phrases [DP-1+PP[P DP-2]. We found different attachment preferences for these two constructions, a high (DP-1) preference for RCs with genitive antecedents and a low (DP-2) preference for PP antecedents. We also found that in the on-line task, even a strongly biased referential context had only a limited effect on these attachment preferences; in particular, context information does not override or reverse these preferences in the self-paced reading experiment. These results suggest that in on-line ambiguity resolution, adult native speakers mainly rely on phrase-structure and lexical information, and to a lesser extent on discourse-level information.

Parsing ambiguous sentences in Greek 3

INTRODUCTION Several studies have shown that sentence processing in adult native speakers is not only influenced by purely phrase-structure-based parsing strategies such as Late Closure or Minimal Attachment, but also by prosodic cues, contextual information, referentiality, etc. It is, however, a matter of controversy which sources of information are available to the parser at any given point in time during the on-line comprehension of a sentence. Broadly speaking, there are two different views. Syntactic-first approaches argue that the initial parse is only guided by phrasestructure-based parsing strategies such as Late Closure or Minimal Attachment (see e.g. Frazier 1987 and subsequent work). On the other hand, several other parsing models, in particular constraint-satisfaction accounts (MacDonald, 1994; Thornton, Gil & MacDonald, 1998; Thornton, MacDonald & Gil 1999) as well as the referential context hypothesis (Altmann & Steedman 1988, Altmann et al., 1992, Crain & Steedman, 1985; Steedman & Altmann, 1989) argue that in addition to phrase-structure, both lexical-semantic and discourse information influence the processing of (temporary) ambiguous sentences at any given point during on-line comprehension. In a previous study (Papadopoulou & Clahsen 2003), we examined RC attachment preferences in native speakers of Greek (as well as in second language learners of Greek) using off-line and online tasks with sentences presented in isolation. Building on this work, we will in the present study investigate the role of context effects for relative clause attachment in Greek native speakers, and how referential context information interacts with lexical cues for ambiguity resolution.

Parsing ambiguous sentences in Greek 4

PREVIOUS STUDIES ON RELATIVE CLAUSE AMBIGUITIES Lexical biases in RC attachment A series of studies indicate that lexical-semantic information affects the parsing of temporarily ambiguous sentences. Consider, for example, relative-clause attachment ambiguities in sentences such as

(1) The doctor recognized [the pupil]DP-1 with [the nurse]DP-2 who was feeling very tired.

Several studies found that when DP-2 is introduced by a thematic preposition, the RC tends to be attached low, i.e. to the nurse in (1); see e.g. DeVincenzi & Job (1993, 1995), Gilboy, Sopena, Clifton & Frazier (1995), Frenck-Mestre & Pynte (2000), Traxler, Pickering & Clifton (1998). Interestingly, this also holds for languages such as Spanish and French in which sentences such as (2) below show high attachment, i.e. a preference for the RC to be attached to the servant in (2):

(2) Someone shot [the servant]DP-1 of [the actress]DP-2 who was on the balcony

Thus, the presence of a thematic preposition such as with or con seems to affect RC attachment preferences, indicating that lexical-semantic and/or thematic factors influence the parsing of (temporary) ambiguities. Frazier & Clifton's (1996) Construal theory is an attempt to capture these facts. They argue that so-called non-primary phrases, i.e. non-obligatory constituents including RC adjuncts, are construed or associated with the closest thematic processing domain.

Parsing ambiguous sentences in Greek 5

That is, when the DP-2 receives a theta-role from a preposition (as in (1) from with), the RC is processed within this thematic domain and is consequently attached low. In this way, Construal theory accounts for the fact that in sentences such as (1) low attachment is preferred across languages. However, in sentences such as (2) the closest thematic processing domain is the entire DP (the servant of the actress), which includes both DP-1 and DP-2. In those cases, the Construal theory postulates that ‘all possible hosts within this domain are evaluated in parallel using a range of information’ (Carreiras & Clifton 1993: 365). Studies on different languages found cross-linguistic differences in RC attachment for sentences such as (2), but not for sentences such as (1).

Context effects on RC ambiguities Previous studies examining context effects in RC attachment have produced mixed results. On the one hand, two studies examining the role of referential information from preceding discourse did not reveal any significant context effects on RC attachment preferences in the DP-of-DP construction, Zagar, Pynte & Rativeau (1997) for French and Desmet, De Baecke & Brysbaert (2002) for Dutch. These results indicate that referential context information is unavailable to the parser, at least during the initial (first pass) parse. Yet, another study (van Berkum, Hagoort & Brown 1999) obtained context effects in on-line ambiguity resolution for very similar kinds of materials.

Zagar et al. (1997) Using a completion and an eye-tracking experiment these authors examined whether RC attachment preferences in French are affected by referential context information. The type of

Parsing ambiguous sentences in Greek 6

context used by Zagar et al. is extra-sentential and is based on the referential context hypothesis (Altmann et al., 1992; Britt, 1994; Britt, Perfetti, Garrod & Rayner, 1992; Crain & Steedman, 1985; Mitchell et al., 1992; Sedivy & Spivey-Knowlton, 1994; Spivey-Knowlton et al., 1994). They tested sentences such as (3), in which the RC qui semblait plus could be interpreted either as a modifier of the first DP (l’avocat) or the second one (la chanteuse):

(3)

Un journaliste aborda l’avocat de la chanteuse qui semblait plus … ‘A journalist approached the barrister of the singer who seemed more…

According to the referential context hypothesis, one would expect that the RC is more likely to be attached to a host that has several referents in a preceding discourse context. Hence, in order to bias participants towards high or low attachment, several referents were introduced for the first or the second noun respectively. Zagar et al. obtained a context effect only with respect to accuracy of responses; specifically, accuracy was greater when the contextual bias was consistent with the attachment. The on-line measures, however, did not produce any effect of context. Instead, they found an overall high attachment preference regardless of context, similarly to what has been found when constructions such as (2) are presented to native speakers of French in isolation (see Mitchell, Cuetos, Zagar, 1990). Zagar et al. (1997) concluded that contextual information influences the final interpretation of a sentence but not the on-line mechanism that guides initial RC attachment. There are, however, some methodological problems with Zagar et al.'s study and the conclusions drawn. High- and low-attachment biasing contexts were not properly balanced in their materials,

Parsing ambiguous sentences in Greek 7

and this may have affected the results. While the high-attachment contexts contained several potential referents for the first noun and only one for the second, the low-attachment contexts contained several potential referents for both nouns. Moreover, Zagar et al.’s pre-test completion experiment revealed only a very small context effect (p.427), suggesting that the contexts may not have been sufficiently biased to affect the participant’s attachment preferences in the on-line task.

Desmet et al. (2002) These authors investigated the influence of referential context on RC attachment preferences in Dutch in a sentence completion study and an eye-tracking experiment. They studied Dutch sentences such as De agenten verhoren de adviseur van de politici die spreekt met een zachte stem ‘The police interrogate the advisor of the politicians who speaks with a soft voice’. Previous studies examining this kind of construction in isolation found a DP-1 preference for Dutch (Brysbaert & Mitchell, 1996; Mitchell et al., 2000). Like Zagar et al. (1997), Desmet et al. presented these sentences in different referential context conditions, with a preceding neutral context, a preceding high-attachment biasing context, and a preceding low-attachment biasing context. The results were similar to those of Zagar et al.: While in the off-line study the participants’ preferences were strongly influenced by the preceding referential context, in the online (eye-tracking) experiment they were only slightly modulated by context information. Most importantly, the reading times revealed a significant high-attachment preference independent of the preceding context. Desmet et al. conclude from these findings that referential context does not influence initial attachment decisions, but plays a role in later phases of sentence processing.

Parsing ambiguous sentences in Greek 8

Note, however, that like in the Zagar et al. study, the materials constructed for the two context conditions were not exactly parallel. Desmet et al. relied on number disambiguation with one of the DPs being singular and the other one plural. In their high-attachment biasing contexts, DP-1 was introduced with the numeral twee ‘two’, whereas in the low-attachment biasing context different kinds of quantifying elements, e.g. enkele ‘some, en groep ‘a group’ to introduce DP-2. It is not clear if and how this asymmetry affected the results.

van Berkum et al. (1999) This study examined complement/relative clause ambiguities in sentences in which a function word such as that could either be a complementizer (John told the girl that he was having trouble with his car) or a relative pronoun (John told the girl that had been calling…) in a reading study measuring event-related brain potentials (ERPs). The critical sentences were preceded by a complementizer-favoring context (with one referent for the girl) or a relativeclause-favoring context (with two referents for the girl). Van Berkum et al. found a P600 effect on the disambiguating word following that, but only when the continuation of the target sentence went against the preceding context. This, they argued, indicates that discourse-level information strongly influences initial sentence parsing. Note, however, that the design of the van Berkum et al. study has been criticized. Brysbaert & Mitchell (2000) pointed out that all possible combinations of contexts and target sentences were presented to the same participants without any filler items, so that participants may have developed artificial response strategies during this task. Moreover, the presentation rate was extremely slow and unnatural (see van Berkum, Hagoort & Brown 2000 for further discussion).

Parsing ambiguous sentences in Greek 9

Taken together, it would certainly be premature to draw any strong conclusions from the abovementioned studies on the role of context effects in relative clause ambiguity resolution. Clearly, further research is needed on different languages.

RELATIVE CLAUSE ATTACHMENT PREFERENCES IN GREEK In a previous study (Papadopoulou & Clahsen 2003), Greek native speakers’ RC attachment preferences were examined by using sentences presented in isolation, i.e. without any preceding referential context. We will briefly summarize the results of this study. Papadopoulou and Clahsen examined sentences such as those in (4) below that contained complex DPs followed by a temporarily ambiguous relative clause introduced by the complementizer pu 'that'. The second potential host DP either carried morphological genitive case (tis kathigitrias 'the teacher' in (4a) and (4b)) or was the complement of the thematic preposition me 'with' (cf. (4c), (4d)). The disambiguating information forcing either DP-1 or DP-2 attachment was provided by gender marking on the participle.

(4)

(a) Enas

Condition Gen-high (gh) kirios

fonakse ton

a-masc-sg-nom man-masc-sg-nom called tis

kathighitrias

fititi

the-masc-sg-acc student-masc-sg-acc

pu itan apoghoitevmenos apo to neo

the-f-sg-gen teacher-f-sg-gen that was disappointed-masc by the new ekpedheftiko sistima. educational system.

Parsing ambiguous sentences in Greek 10

(= A man called the student (masc) of the teacher (fem) who was disappointed (masc) by the new educational system.) (b)

Condition Gen-low (gl)

Enas kirios fonakse ton fititi tis kathighitrias pu itan apoghoitevmeni apo to neo ekpedheftiko sistima. (= A man called the student (masc) of the teacher (fem) who was disappointed (fem) by the new educational system.) (c)

Condition PP-high (ph)

Enas kirios fonakse ton fititi me tin kathighitria pu itan apoghoitevmenos apo to neo ekpedheftiko sistima. (= A man called the student (masc) with the teacher (fem) who was disappointed (masc) by the new educational system.) (d)

Condition PP-low (pl)

Enas kirios fonakse ton fititi me tin kathighitria pu itan apoghoitevmeni apo to neo ekpedheftiko sistima. (= A man called the student (masc) with the teacher (fem) who was disappointed (fem) by the new educational system.)

Papadopoulou and Clahsen found that in both off-line and on-line tasks, native speakers of Greek showed a a high-attachment preference in sentences with genitive antecedents (4a vs. 4b). In sentences with PP antecedents (4c vs. 4d), on the other hand, they obtained a low-attachment preference for the RC, both off-line and on-line. This latter finding replicates previous results on other languages showing that when the second DP is introduced by a theta-role assigning lexical

Parsing ambiguous sentences in Greek 11

preposition, the RC tends to be attached low (Gilboy et al.,1995; Frenck-Mestre & Pynte,2000; Traxler et al., 1998).

THE PRESENT STUDY This study is designed to address the question of how lexical-semantic cues interact with referential context information in ambiguity resolution. Two relative clause (RC) constructions of Greek were examined, (i) RCs preceded by complex noun phrases with genitives [DP+DPGen] and (ii) RCs preceded by complex noun phrases containing prepositional phrases [DP+PP[P DP]. These two constructions were tested in different context conditions (with and without biasing context) in an off-line completion task and a self-paced reading experiment. According to constraint-satisfaction models of parsing (MacDonald 1994, among others) and the referential context hypothesis (see e.g. Altman & Steedman, 1988) all sources of information, including referential context information should affect parsing. Thornton et al. (1998; 1999), for example, claimed that the felicity of DP modification determines RC attachment preferences such that a DP is more likely to be further modified if it has not received much modification. With our experimental materials and tasks, these predictions can be tested. If referential context affects both initial and second-pass parsing, then we would expect to find the same context/felicity effects in both experimental tasks and in both constructions. From the perspective of syntax-first approaches to parsing (Frazier 1987, among others), one would expect referential context information to influence the final interpretation of a sentence, i.e. second-pass parsing but not the on-line mechanism that guides RC attachment. If this is correct, we should find strong context effects in the off-line task but not in the on-line one.

Parsing ambiguous sentences in Greek 12

EXPERIMENT 1: COMPLETION STUDY The main purpose of this experiment is to make sure that the contexts used are biased enough to affect RC attachment preferences. Three context conditions were examined: a neutral condition in which the experimental sentences were presented in isolation without any preceding context, a high-attachment biasing context, and a low-attachment biasing context. In the neutral condition, we expect to replicate Papadopoulou & Clahsen’s (2003) results on Greek RC attachment, i.e. a high attachment preference for DP+DPgenitive and a low-attachment one for DP+PP. Context effects would be evident if the initial DP-1 preference for genitives is magnified by a highattachment biasing context and neutralized or reversed by a low-attachment biasing context. Likewise, the initial DP-2 preference for PP antecedents should be strengthened by a lowattachment biasing context and wiped out or reversed in a high-attachment biasing context.

METHOD Participants Forty adult native speakers of Greek, all of them students of the University of Athens, participated voluntarily in the experiment. There were twenty participants in the no-context version (in which sentences were presented in isolation) and twenty participants in the version with preceding context, none of whom participated in both versions.

Materials and design Twenty-four experimental sentences were constructed1. The experimental sentences contained a RC with two possible antecedents. The RCs were always subject-RCs introduced by the 1

A complete list of all experimental stimuli for each task can be found in Papadopoulou (2002) and can be made available upon request.

Parsing ambiguous sentences in Greek 13

complementiser pu ‘that’. The two DPs preceding the RC were always animate, had different gender (either feminine or masculine) and involved a functional/professional relationship. Two versions of each experimental sentence were constructed so that they differed only with respect to the form of the complex DP that preceded the RC. In one version, DP-2 appeared in genitive case, whereas the second one contained a PP introduced by the lexical preposition me ‘with’; see (5) and (6) for examples. Participants were presented with sentence fragments such as (5a) or (5b) and had to choose between a low (6a) and a high (6b) attachment continuation of the sentence fragment. Note that the sentence fragments are ambiguous up to the auxiliary itan ‘was’, and that in the sentence continuations, the ambiguity is resolved by means of gender information on a past participle, which agrees either with the first or the second noun, resulting in high or low RC attachment.

(5) a. Enas dhimosioghrafos kitakse ti mathitria tu dhaskalu pu itan… 'A journalist looked at the pupil-fem the-gen teacher-masc-gen that was…' b. Enas dhimosioghrafos kitakse ti mathitria me to dhaskalo pu itan… 'A journalist looked at the pupil-fem with the teacher-masc that was…'

(6) a. …nevriasmenos

eksetias

tis

kathisterisis

…angry-nom-sg-masc because-of the-gen -sg-fem delay-gen-fem b. …nevriasmeni

eksetias

tis

…angry-nom-sg-fem because-of the-gen -sg-fem

kathisterisis delay-gen-fem

Parsing ambiguous sentences in Greek 14

The RCs were selected so that both DPs preceding the RC could be equally plausible hosts for the RC on semantic and pragmatic grounds. In addition to the experimental sentences there were Forty-eight filler sentences involving a variety of constructions. Half of the fillers were ambiguous and half were unambiguous. To investigate context effects, seventy-two texts were constructed, twenty-four to precede the experimental sentences and forty-eight for the filler sentences. For the experimental sentences, referential context was used to bias participants towards high or low attachment. In the text preceding each experimental sentence, two potential referents were introduced either for the first or the second antecedent of the RC, as illustrated in (7).

(7) a. Context biased towards high attachment The schoolyard was crowded. Children, parents and teachers were waiting for the celebration to start. It was 10.30 and the celebration was supposed to have started at 10. One pupil was talking with two of her teachers. The pupil and one of the teachers were really upset. The other teacher was listening to the conversation skeptically. b. Context biased towards low attachment The schoolyard was crowded. Children, parents and teachers were waiting for the celebration to start. It was 10.30 and the celebration was supposed to have started at 10. Two pupils were talking with their teacher. The teacher and one of the pupils were really upset. The other pupil was listening to the conversation skeptically.

Hence, the biasing contexts constructed for the present study were properly balanced in that low and high attachment biasing contexts had two referents for the second and the first noun

Parsing ambiguous sentences in Greek 15

respectively. The filler sentences were also preceded by texts of the same length as the experimental sentences. For half of the filler texts, only one completion of the target sentence was appropriate given the semantic and pragmatic information provided in the preceding text. For the other half, the context biased the participants towards one of the two completions. As mentioned above, the form of the complex DP that preceded the RC was also manipulated, parallel to our earlier study (see (4) above), yielding four experimental conditions.

(8) RC antecedent

Context

DP+DPgenitive

High-attachment biasing Low-attachment biasing

DP+PP

High-attachment biasing Low-attachment biasing

Procedure Two experimental versions were constructed, one in which the critical (and the filler) sentences were presented in isolation, i.e. without any preceding context, and one with preceding context. In the no-context version, two booklets of seventy-two sentences each were presented to the participants in which each sentence fragment together with two possible continuations was typed on separate sheets of paper. Each booklet contained the same number of fillers and experimental sentences, twelve of which had DP+DPgen and twelve DP+PP antecedents for the RCs, and the same sentence was not seen more than once by any participant. In half of the experimental sentences, the DP-1 attachment interpretation was shown as the first completion, and in the other half it was shown as the second one. The participants were instructed to read the sentences only once and to circle the option that seemed most appropriate to them as a completion of the

Parsing ambiguous sentences in Greek 16

sentence. In the experimental version with preceding context, booklets with the same seventytwo sentences were constructed in which the critical (and filler) sentences were preceded by short texts. Each short text was printed on a separate sheet of paper followed (on the same page) by an experimental or filler sentence. The participants’ task was again to circle the option that seemed most appropriate to them as a completion of the sentence. Four different booklets were constructed ensuring that each one contained all the conditions tested and that each participant was exposed to all conditions. Participants were tested in two sessions in each of which the same experimental sentences were presented with different preceding contexts (biased towards high or low attachment). There was a two-week interval between the two sessions.

Results Table 1 presents percentages of high (= DP-1) and low (= DP-2) continuations provided by the participants2. //INSERT TABLE 1 ABOUT HERE// In the condition in which the experimental sentences were presented in isolation, Tab.1 shows a significant high-attachment preference for genitives (t1(19) = 4.945, p