Predicting Cross-Cultural Training Performance - CiteSeerX

1 downloads 272 Views 114KB Size Report
and dimensions measured by assessment center exercises and a behavior description interview. Results show that ... M. Harris, College of Business Administration, University of Missouri—. St. Louis; Etienne .... (expatriate perceptions, self-development, relations with host ... The learning in the ETP typically occurs in small.
Journal of Applied Psychology 2003, Vol. 88, No. 3, 476 – 489

Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0021-9010/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.3.476

Predicting Cross-Cultural Training Performance: The Validity of Personality, Cognitive Ability, and Dimensions Measured by an Assessment Center and a Behavior Description Interview Filip Lievens

Michael M. Harris

Ghent University

University of Missouri—St. Louis

Etienne Van Keer

Claire Bisqueret

TMP Worldwide

SHL Belgium

This study examined the validity of a broad set of predictors for selecting European managers for a cross-cultural training program in Japan. The selection procedure assessed cognitive ability, personality, and dimensions measured by assessment center exercises and a behavior description interview. Results show that the factor Openness was significantly related to cross-cultural training performance, whereas cognitive ability was significantly correlated with language acquisition. The dimensions of adaptability, teamwork, and communication as measured by a group discussion exercise provided incremental variance in both criteria, beyond cognitive ability and personality. In general, these results are consistent with the literature on domestic selection, although there are some important differences.

improving managers’ cross-cultural effectiveness and for reducing failure rates (Bhagat & Prien, 1996; Bhawuk & Brislin, 2000; Black & Mendenhall, 1989; Deshpande & Viswesvaran, 1992; Dowling et al., 1999; Earley, 1987). It seems reasonable, therefore, to begin by selecting managers on the basis of their capacity to successfully master the cross-cultural training. Along these lines, Caligiuri (2000) asserted that practitioners should think of expatriate selection as the precursor to cross-cultural training. She further argued that organizations “should identify those expatriate candidates with the requisite personality characteristics, and then offer cross-cultural training to those identified. Cross-cultural training may only be effective when the expatriates are predisposed to success in the first place” (Caligiuri, 2000, p. 85). However, recent reviews have indicated that the selection process of international managers is still intuitive and unsystematic (Deller, 1997; Ones & Viswesvaran, 1997; Sinangil & Ones, 2001). One of the problems is that the selection of people for foreign assignments is often based solely on job knowledge and technical competence (Aryee, 1997; Schmitt & Chan, 1998; Sinangil & Ones, 2001; Tung, 1981). Another problem is that in the past, researchers have mainly tried to determine a list of (inter) personal factors responsible for expatriate adjustment versus failure (e.g., Mendenhall & Oddou, 1985; Ones & Viswesvaran, 1997; Ronen, 1989) but that “there is apparently little research on designing selection systems which might be effective predictors of success in overseas assignments” (Arvey, Bhagat, & Salas, 1991, p. 378). More generally, it has been stated that there is a clear need to improve and broaden existing techniques for selecting people for international assignments (Arthur & Bennett, 1997; Church, 1982; Hough & Oswald, 2000). This need for more international selection research is especially striking in light of the large research base about predictors in traditional domestic selection (Schmidt & Hunter, 1998).

The cost of managers on foreign assignments is extremely high for organizations. When such benefits as housing, airfare, and special premiums are included, the typical compensation package is estimated to cost at least 2.5 times an expatriate’s salary, and a significant number of companies indicate that the cost may be 4 to 5 times the base salary (Dowling, Welch, & Schuler, 1999). Moreover, the costs (e.g., in lost business, preparation, and so forth) associated with the failure of an expatriate employee are extremely high and have been estimated to range between $200,000 and $1.2 million (Duane, 2001). Because of the high stakes involved, organizations have been looking for ways to improve the adjustment of managers to foreign countries. One approach has been to focus on selection, which has begun to receive some research attention (Caligiuri, 2000; Deller, 1997; Ones & Viswesvaran, 1997; Spreitzer, McCall, & Mahoney, 1997). A second approach has been to invest in cross-cultural training, which has been identified as another major technique for

Filip Lievens, Department of Personnel Management and Work and Organizational Psychology, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium; Michael M. Harris, College of Business Administration, University of Missouri— St. Louis; Etienne Van Keer, TMP Worldwide, Ghent, Belgium; Claire Bisqueret, SHL Belgium, Brussels, Belgium. A previous version of this article was presented at the annual meeting of the Academy of Management, Denver, Colorado, August 2002. We acknowledge Dennis Briscoe, Wayne Cascio, Paul Sackett, and Neal Schmitt for their valuable suggestions on an earlier version of this article. We also thank Francisco Garcia, Diederik Paalman, and Fabrice Vareille for their permission to use the Executive Training Program data for scientific purposes. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Filip Lievens, Department of Personnel Management and Work and Organizational Psychology, Ghent University, Henri Dunantlaan 2, 9000 Ghent, Belgium. E-mail: [email protected] 476

PREDICTORS OF CROSS-CULTURAL TRAINING PERFORMANCE

To respond to this gap in the literature, in this study we sought to examine the validity of four commonly used predictors, namely, cognitive ability, personality, and dimensions measured by assessment center exercises and a behavior description interview for selection into a cross-cultural training program. This study extends existing international selection research in three important ways. First, in response to the limited amount of research on expatriate selection, we broaden existing methods for selecting people for international assignments by including a wide range of predictors. Second, we examine the incremental validity of various predictors in an international context. Specifically, we focus on the incremental validity of dimensions measured in assessment center exercises and a behavior description interview over cognitive ability and personality in predicting cross-cultural training performance. Third, we examine a relatively unique criterion, namely, performance in cross-cultural training. Whereas various selection techniques have been used to predict training performance within domestic programs, there is almost no research with regard to their validity in predicting training performance in a cross-cultural context, despite the growing practical and scientific importance of cross-cultural training. In the next sections, we review the relevant literature regarding the predictors used and formulate hypotheses on the basis of research in the domestic context. Beforehand, however, we discuss the practical and scientific importance of cross-cultural training and address why predictors of cross-cultural training success may differ from predictors of success in traditional training. We also briefly describe the cross-cultural training program under investigation.

Importance of Cross-Cultural Training Cross-cultural training has long been proposed as an anticipatory mechanism to increase adjustment to foreign cultures (Mendenhall & Oddou, 1985; Tung, 1981). In cross-cultural training programs, a wide variety of training methods are typically used, including lectures, video films, experiential exercises, culture assimilators, and behavior modification (Bhawuk & Brislin, 2000; Black & Mendenhall, 1989; Mendenhall & Stahl, 2000). Bhagat and Prien (1996) discussed the main differences between traditional training and cross-cultural training. As they described, traditional training is characterized by a focus on the “acquisition of information, rather than on change in attitudes” (Bhagat & Prien, 1996, p. 223). By way of comparison, Bhagat and Prien observed that cross-cultural training addresses the acceptance of differences between cultures. Several recent surveys (cited in Sinangil & Ones, 2001) illustrate that cross-cultural training is increasingly used as a staffing practice in international human resources management. A survey by Andersen Consulting, for example, revealed that among the best 32 of Fortune 500 organizations, 94% offered language training and 69% offered other cross-cultural training to international assignees. Sinangil and Ones (2001) cited another recent survey of 250 companies, showing that approximately 63% offered crosscultural training to their expatriates. These usage percentages are much higher than those reported in earlier surveys, in which usage was estimated at around 25% (e.g., Black & Gregersen, 1991). Furthermore, empirical research evidence attests to the effectiveness of cross-cultural training as a means to facilitate crosscultural interactions in the foreign country. Black and Mendenhall

477

(1990) reviewed 29 empirical studies that evaluated the effectiveness of various cross-cultural training programs. Their comprehensive literature review showed that cross-cultural training had a strong positive impact on participants’ self-confidence, on their interpersonal relationships with host nationals, and on their perceptions of the host culture. Equally important, Black and Mendenhall’s review (1990) confirmed the positive influence of crosscultural training on expatriate adjustment and expatriate performance. Deshpande and Viswesvaran (1992) used metaanalysis to investigate the effectiveness of cross-cultural training. Across 21 empirical studies, a total of 1,611 participants were used to examine the effects of cross-cultural training on five criteria (expatriate perceptions, self-development, relations with host country nationals, adjustment, and performance). Results demonstrated the effectiveness of cross-cultural training as a key strategy for increasing expatriate success. For example, there was a corrected correlation between cross-cultural training and expatriate job performance of .39. Since this meta-analysis was published, other primary empirical studies have further confirmed the effectiveness of cross-cultural training programs (Bhawuk, 1998; Hammer & Martin, 1992; Harrison, 1992). Given this growing practical and scientific value of crosscultural training, it is important to select expatriates for participation in cross-cultural training (Caligiuri, 2000). Although there exists an extensive literature on the validity of different tools for selection into traditional training programs (Olea & Ree, 1994; Ree & Earles, 1991; Ree, Carretta, & Teachout, 1995; Salas & Cannon-Bowers, 2001; see also Table 2 in Schmidt & Hunter, 1998), there have been no studies about the predictors of success in a cross-cultural training program. Moreover, given the different requirements for success in traditional versus cross-cultural training (see Bhagat & Prien, 1996), it seems likely that the predictors may differ. As asserted by Hough and Oswald (2000), “validities of domestic selection instruments may not generalize to international sites, because different predictor and criterion constructs may be relevant, or, if the constructs are the same, the behavioral indicators may differ” (p. 649).

The Executive Training Program in Japan The Executive Training Program (ETP) in Japan is a crosscultural training program aimed at providing European managers with an in-depth understanding of Japanese business-related practices (including Japanese culture and society) and language. The European Commission financially supports the ETP and accordingly aims to facilitate European companies’ access to the Japanese market. The program consists of intensive language courses, university seminars, company visits, and in-house training. Thus, the ETP appears to be a blend of the experiential (e.g., field trips, role-playing) and analytical training methods (e.g., language training, case studies) in Black and Mendenhall’s (1989) cross-cultural training model. The learning in the ETP typically occurs in small groups (e.g., language learning occurs in groups of three). During 12 months, participants are familiarized with the Japanese language and with Japanese business and management. After these 12 months, participants work in Japanese host companies for 6 months. During these 6 months, executives work with Japanese coworkers and supervisors on various projects. Accordingly, it provides them with an opportunity to demonstrate that they have

LIEVENS, HARRIS, VAN KEER, AND BISQUERET

478

acquired the language and business-related skills necessary to perform successfully in Japan. The purpose of the selection procedure was to identify managers who would successfully complete the ETP in Japan. To determine the specific performance dimensions related to managers’ performance in the ETP in Japan, we began by holding a series of workshops with the ETP staff. The choice of the criteria was based on (a) the objectives of the ETP (see above), (b) a review of the current role requirements and skills present in successful participants of the ETP, and (c) a review of the extant literature on expatriate adjustment. Besides mastery of the Japanese language,

seven performance dimensions were specified to represent the business-related component of cross-cultural training performance: self-discipline, tenacity–resilience, teamwork, communication, adaptability, cross-cultural awareness, and organizational and commercial awareness (see Table 1, first column). We also conducted workshops with the ETP staff to develop specific behavioral definitions of these seven dimensions (see Table 1). Inspection of these performance dimensions confirms the aforementioned assertions about the distinctions between traditional training and cross-cultural training (Bhagat & Prien, 1996). In fact, some of the dimensions might be relevant for any training pro-

Table 1 Overview of Criteria, Predictor Constructs, Predictor Instruments, and Their Hypothesized Linkages Predictor constructs and instruments Criteria (performance dimensions)

Assessment center exercises and behavior description interview

Big Five factors and selected OPQ scales Emotional Stability Relaxed (calm, relaxed, cool under pressure, free from anxiety, can switch off) ● Tough-minded (difficult to hurt or upset, can brush off insults, unaffected by unfair remarks)

● ●

Analysis–presentation (tenacity–resilience rating) Behavior description interview (tenacity– resilience rating)

Extraversion ● Affiliative (has many friends, enjoys being in groups, likes companionship, shares things with friends) ● Outgoing (fun-loving, humorous, sociable, vibrant, talkative, jovial)

● ●

Analysis–presentation (communication rating) Group discussion (communication rating)



Openness Change oriented (enjoys doing new things, seeks variety, prefers novelty to routine, accepts changes) ● Independent (has strong views on things, is difficult to manage, speaks up, argues, dislikes ties)

● ●

Analysis–presentation (adaptability rating) Group discussion (adaptability rating)

Agreeableness Democratic (encourages others to contribute, consults, listens, and refers to others)

● ● ●

Group discussion (teamwork rating) Behavior description interview (teamwork rating) Behavior description interview (self-discipline rating)

Cross-cultural awareness (is able to see issues from the perspective of people of other cultures)



Behavior description interview (cross-cultural awareness rating)

Organizational and commercial awareness (is alert to changing organizational dynamics, is knowledgeable about financial and commercial issues, focuses on markets, and business opportunities that bring the largest return)



Analysis–presentation (organizational and commercial awareness rating) Group discussion (organizational and commercial awareness rating)

Tenacity–resilience (keeps difficulties in perspective, stays positive despite disappointments and setbacks)

Communication (is able to communicate clearly, fluently, and to the point; talks at a pace and level that holds people’s attention, both in group and individual situations) Adaptability (adapts readily to new situations and ways of working, receptive to new ideas, willing and able to adjust to changing demands and objectives)

Teamwork (cooperates and works well with others in the pursuit of team goals, shares information, develops supportive relationships with colleagues and creates a sense of team spirit) Self-discipline (is committed, consistent, and dependable; can be relied on to deliver what has been agreed; is punctual and conscientious)





Conscientiousness Conscientious (sticks to deadlines, completes jobs, perseveres with routine, likes fixed schedules) ● Achieving (ambitious, sets sights high, career centered, results oriented) ●



Note. If two subscales measured a Big Five factor, subscale scores were averaged to obtain a score on the Big Five factor (see Caligiuri, 2000). OPQ ⫽ Occupational Personality Questionnaire.

PREDICTORS OF CROSS-CULTURAL TRAINING PERFORMANCE

gram. Examples include self-discipline, tenacity–resilience, teamwork, and communication. However, other performance dimensions (e.g., language proficiency, adaptability, and cross-cultural awareness) would not typically be critical in a domestic training program. Among these dimensions, language proficiency deals with the acquisition of information, whereas adaptability and cross-cultural awareness pertain to the acceptance of differences.

Validity of Predictors Cognitive Ability General cognitive ability, or g, has been found to be a consistent predictor of job performance across a variety of occupations (Schmidt & Hunter, 1998). This is especially the case for more complex job levels (Hunter, 1986). In addition, general cognitive ability has been identified as a key determinant of learning and skill acquisition (Kanfer & Ackerman, 1989; Salas & CannonBowers, 2001). This is evidenced by several large-scale studies in military settings, which found a strong relationship between cognitive ability and training performance (Olea & Ree, 1994; Ree et al., 1995; Ree & Earles, 1991). Finally, LePine, Colquitt, and Erez (2000) demonstrated that cognitive ability was an important determinant of effective coping with unforeseen changes in the task context. Thus, these studies in the domestic context show that general cognitive ability is consistently and strongly related to job and training performance and that this relationship increases in more complex, novel, and changing tasks. Because international managers are required to learn and to deal with a variety of novel and complex situations in a cross-cultural training program (Spreitzer et al., 1997), cognitive ability measures were included in this study. In particular, managers’ general cognitive ability was estimated with tests of verbal and numerical reasoning. We chose these subtests because they are indicators of general cognitive ability (Ree et al., 1995). We expected that general cognitive ability would be significantly related to crosscultural training performance (Hypothesis 1).

Personality Over the past decade, the use of the five-factor model has served as a unifying theoretical framework to substantially advance our understanding of personality-based predictors in the context of domestic selection (Barrick & Mount, 1991; Hough, Eaton, Dunnette, Kamp, & McCloy, 1990; Hurtz & Donovan, 2000; Salgado, 1997; Tett, Jackson, & Rothstein, 1991). Although Barrick and Mount’s (1991) meta-analysis of the Big Five personality dimensions focused on Conscientiousness as a predictor of job performance, their meta-analytic results also supported the validity of personality for predicting training performance. Specifically, in addition to Conscientiousness, both Extraversion (corrected r ⫽ .26) and Openness (corrected r ⫽ .25) exhibited sufficiently high validity coefficients with training proficiency to warrant further examination (Salas & Cannon-Bowers, 2001). In an international context, personality-based predictors have also known a renaissance. Although prior research produced discouraging findings (see Ones & Viswesvaran, 1997, for an excellent review), recent studies indeed have found more promising validity results for the use of the five-factor model in a global

479

context (Caligiuri, 2000; Dalton & Wilson, 2000; Schmit, Kihm, & Robie, 2000). In a concurrent validity study, Caligiuri (2000) concluded that all Big Five factors (with the exception of Openness) had statistically significant negative relationships with expatriates’ desire to prematurely terminate the assignment. In addition, Emotional Stability was significantly related to expatriates’ performance as rated by supervisors. Dalton and Wilson (2000) examined the relationship of the five-factor model to job performance ratings of 21 Middle Eastern expatriate managers. Both Conscientiousness and Agreeableness were related to homecountry boss ratings but not to host-country boss ratings. With input from psychologists around the world, Schmit et al. (2000) constructed a “global” personality inventory based on the fivefactor model. Initial predictive validity evidence was presented, showing that Conscientiousness, Extraversion, and Openness were significant predictors of job performance. Because there seems to be an emerging consensus about the usefulness of the five-factor model in both domestic and international selection, three hypotheses were proposed. First, we hypothesized that Openness would be a significant predictor for managers’ cross-cultural training performance (Hypothesis 2a). This hypothesis was based on the fact that meta-analyses have shown that Openness is related to training performance in the domestic context (Barrick & Mount, 1991; Salgado, 1997). The significant relationship between Openness and training performance may be explained by the fact that open individuals are intellectually curious and independent thinkers who seek out new and unconventional experiences (Barrick & Mount, 1991). Accordingly, Openness may help people to better deal with the acceptance of differences between cultures, which is a prime component of cross-cultural training (Bhagat & Prien, 1996). Research has also shown that people high on Openness are more ready to engage in new learning experiences (Blickle, 1996), adapt more adequately to novel and unforeseen changes (LePine et al., 2000), and adjust more rapidly to a radically different work environment (Bing & Lounsbury, 2000). Similarly, Barrick and Mount (1991) found that Extraversion had a relatively large correlation with training performance. Individuals who are friendly and outgoing should be more likely to engage in interactive activities and therefore should be more successful in training. Also, recall that the training program used here relied heavily on working in small groups, so that Extraversion should enable trainees to be more effective in this setting. Hence, we hypothesized that Extraversion would be a significant predictor for managers’ cross-cultural training performance (Hypothesis 2b). Finally, Conscientiousness has been studied in relation to training programs and has been found to be an important predictor (e.g., Colquitt & Simmering, 1998). Trainees high on Conscientiousness are likely to be high on self-efficacy, motivation to learn, and other important training performance determinants (Colquitt, LePine, & Noe, 2000). On that basis, we hypothesized that Conscientiousness would be a significant predictor for managers’ cross-cultural training performance (Hypothesis 2c).

Dimensions Measured by Assessment Center Exercises In the domestic context, assessment centers have been found to be good predictors of a variety of managerial criteria such as job and training performance (Gaugler, Rosenthal, Thornton, & Bent-

480

LIEVENS, HARRIS, VAN KEER, AND BISQUERET

son, 1987; Pynes & Bernardin, 1989). The meta-analysis of Gaugler et al. (1987) found a corrected validity coefficient of .35 for predicting training performance. In addition, a meta-analysis of work-sample tests of trainability (trainability tests) showed these tests to be good predictors of training success across a variety of jobs (Robertson & Downs, 1989). Yet an unanswered question is whether assessment centers are also predictive if used for international applications (Arthur & Bennett, 1997; Briscoe, 1997; Love, Bishop, Heinisch, & Montei, 1994; Ones & Viswesvaran, 1997). Because assessment center exercises are methods that can be used to measure a wide array of constructs (Arthur, Day, McNelly, & Edens, in press), it should also be possible to design specific assessment center exercises to measure dimensions related to performance in an international context. Hence, we constructed two assessment center exercises for measuring various dimensions related to cross-cultural training performance (see third column of Table 1). Specifically, an analysis–presentation exercise was designed to measure the dimensions of tenacity–resilience, communication, adaptability, and organizational and commercial awareness. Next, a group discussion exercise was constructed to capture the dimensions of teamwork, communication, adaptability, and organizational and commercial awareness. We hypothesized that the dimensions measured by these assessment center exercises would predict managers’ cross-cultural training performance (Hypothesis 3).

Dimensions Measured by High-Structure Interviews Over the past decade, high-structure interviews have emerged as valid predictors of job performance in the domestic context (Huffcutt & Arthur, 1994; McDaniel, Whetzel, Schmidt, & Maurer, 1994; Wiesner & Cronshaw, 1988). Two types of these interviews have dominated, namely, behavior description interviews (Janz, 1982) and situational interviews (Latham, Saari, Pursell, & Campion, 1980). Besides being predictive of job performance, there is research suggesting that high-structure interviews are predictive of training performance. McDaniel et al. (1994) reported a correlation of .21 (corrected r ⫽ .34) between high-structure interviews and training performance, which was practically identical to the average validity of the interview for job performance. Contrary to this research in the domestic context, research on high-structure interviews in a cross-cultural context is scarce. An exception is Stahl (1995), who developed a high-structure interview for selecting managers for an assignment in Japan. Yet the sample was small (N ⫽ 8), and no criterion-related validity evidence was gathered. In this study, we included a behavior description interview in the selection procedure. Recent research comparing the behavior description interview and situational interview has shown that the former is more valid for higher level positions (Huffcutt, Weekley, Wiesner, DeGroot, & Jones, 2001; Pulakos & Schmitt, 1995). Because a behavior description interview is essentially a method that can be used to measure a variety of constructs (Huffcutt, Conway, Roth, & Stone, 2001), it should also be possible to design a behavior description interview to measure dimensions related to performance in an international context. Therefore, we constructed a behavior description interview to measure several dimensions relevant to cross-cultural training performance (see third column of Table 1). In this study, a behavior description interview was developed around the dimensions of self-discipline, tenacity–

resilience, teamwork, and cross-cultural awareness. We hypothesized that the dimensions measured by this behavior description interview would predict managers’ cross-cultural training performance (Hypothesis 4).

Incremental Validity Apart from each predictor’s validity, it is both theoretically and practically pivotal to examine the predictive validity of different predictors over and above each other. From a theoretical viewpoint, there is a need to understand whether these different measures are actually measuring similar or different constructs. From a practical standpoint, knowing whether to add additional predictors that will explain additional variance in the criteria can help fine-tune a selection process. Typically referred to as incremental validity, the use of additional predictors is of value from a utility viewpoint only when they add additional variance explained in the criterion, beyond that which is accounted for by other, less expensive predictors (Schmidt & Hunter, 1998). Because the validity of cognitive ability and personality (i.e., Conscientiousness) has been well established and because these predictors are less expensive, we examined whether dimensions measured by assessment center exercises and a behavior description interview provide incremental validity beyond these two predictors in explaining cross-cultural training performance. In recent years, this specific incremental validity question has been addressed by some studies in the domestic context. In terms of the incremental validity of assessment centers over personality measures, research is scarce. Goffin, Rothstein, and Johnston (1996) reported almost no correlation between personality and assessment center ratings. Hence, both personality and assessment center ratings had significant incremental validity over one another. There is somewhat more research on the relationship between assessment centers and cognitive ability. In their comprehensive review, Schmidt and Hunter (1998) included an earlier meta-analysis that found a high correlation (r ⫽ .50) between assessment center ratings and cognitive ability. They noted, however, that in this meta-analysis some assessment centers incorporated a cognitive ability measure, and so in certain instances, there might be contamination between assessment center ratings and cognitive ability. H. W. Goldstein, Yusko, Braverman, Smith, and Chung (1998) argued that the relationship varied as a function of the cognitive “loading” of assessment center exercises. That is, when exercises (e.g., an in-basket exercise) tapped more cognitiveoriented dimensions (e.g., problem analysis), there was a stronger relationship between the assessment center exercise and cognitive ability. In support of this hypothesis, Goldstein et al. found exercise– cognitive ability test score correlations ranging between .09 and .29. Yet these are relatively modest correlations, and only one of the five exercises became statistically unrelated to performance when cognitive ability was partialed out. Thus, Goldstein et al.’s results suggested that dimensions measured in assessment center exercises would provide incremental validity over cognitive ability, even with moderate correlations between these two predictors. There is a relatively large literature examining the incremental validity of high-structure interviews over cognitive ability. Although earlier studies suggested that the high-structure interview measured little more than cognitive ability (e.g., Campion, Pursell,

PREDICTORS OF CROSS-CULTURAL TRAINING PERFORMANCE

& Brown, 1988), Huffcutt, Roth, and McDaniel’s (1996) metaanalysis reported a correlation of .12 (corrected r ⫽ .18) between behavior description interviews and cognitive ability. In terms of the relationship between interview ratings and personality, most of the research has focused on Conscientiousness. Cortina, Goldstein, Payne, Davison, and Gilliland (2000) concluded that the correlation between interview ratings and other constructs varied, depending on the amount of structure imposed on the interview. At higher levels of structure, they found an average correlation of .21 (corrected r ⫽ .26) between interview ratings and Conscientiousness. The relationship between interview ratings and other Big Five factors (e.g., Agreeableness) has been the focus of far less research. In one recent study, however, Huffcutt, Weekley, Wiesner, DeGroot, and Jones (2001) reported small correlations between the Big Five and a behavior description interview, with the exception of a moderate correlation for Extraversion (r ⫽ .30). In sum, prior research in the domestic context about the incremental validity of dimensions measured by assessment center exercises and a behavior description interview over cognitive ability and personality shows that there are at best moderate correlations among these predictors. Therefore, we hypothesized that dimensions measured by assessment center exercises and a behavior description interview would add incremental validity over and beyond personality and cognitive ability (Hypothesis 5).

Method Sample One hundred sixty-six European managers participated in the selection procedure. The sample included 125 men and 41 women. Participants ranged in age from 23 to 52 years, with an average age of 29.3 years (SD ⫽ 4.4 years). In total, 15 European nationalities were involved. With regard to job titles, the sample was relatively heterogeneous. Most managers were active in export, marketing, sales, or general management. In terms of managerial level, many were at the executive level, although others were high-potential employees who were judged likely to become executives. They were working in a variety of companies (services, manufacturing, electronics, etc.). Most of the companies (71%) were doing business with Japan. Forty percent were small companies (fewer than 50 employees), 20% were medium-sized companies (between 50 and 250 employees), and 38% were large companies (more than 250 employees). The preselection of the managers had taken place in their home countries by local divisions of the consultancy firm responsible for the actual selection. This preselection was done on the basis of relevant prior experience and other biographical information (e.g., 2 years of managerial experience, 6 months of international experience). The actual selection procedure lasted 1 day. In total, 30 sessions were held. Each session was attended by four to six managers (of differing nationalities; see Briscoe, 1997), who were evaluated by experienced psychologists (i.e., external consultants) and staff members of the ETP.

Predictor Measures Cognitive ability measures. The first test (VMG 1) was a verbal critical reasoning test that consisted of 52 items describing short passages of complex and business-related information (Saville & Holdsworth, 1989). The questions asked candidates to evaluate whether specific statements were congruent with the information provided. The time limit was 25 min. The second test (NMG 1) was a numerical critical-reasoning test that consisted of 40 items depicting business-related tables and graphs (time limit: 35 min; Saville & Holdsworth, 1989). Candidates were required to

481

interpret and use this complex numerical information to answer the questions. Candidates completed these tests in their own language. Because these cognitive ability measures were the publisher’s property, we received only candidates’ final scores and were not able to compute internal consistencies. Prior research mentioned in the test manual, however, indicated internal consistencies varying (across different studies) from .70 to .80 for VMG 1 and from .82 to .90 for NMG 1. Across different validation studies and criteria, validity coefficients ranged from .17 ( p ⬍ .05, N ⫽ 109) to .37 ( p ⬍ .05, N ⫽ 38). The correlation between VMG 1 and NMG 1 scores was .38 ( p ⬍ .01). We averaged scores on the two tests to obtain a measure of general cognitive ability (see Ree et al., 1995). Personality inventory. To measure the Big Five personality factors, we selected subscales from the Occupational Personality Questionnaire (OPQ; Saville & Holdsworth, 1990). Although the OPQ was originally not designed to measure the five-factor model, various studies (e.g., Beaujouan, 2000; Ferguson, Payne, & Anderson, 1994; Matthews & Stanton, 1994; Matthews, Stanton, Graham, & Brimelow, 1990) have shown that the subscales of the OPQ can be retranslated to the five-factor model. For example, Matthews et al. (1990) demonstrated that the majority of covariance among the OPQ scales could be explained by five factors, which corresponded well to the five-factor model. Other studies have found that the OPQ consistently predicted job performance across organizations, nations, and time (Robertson & Kinder, 1993; Saville, Sik, Nyfield, Hackston, & MacIver, 1996). On the basis of these prior studies, we chose nine subscales and linked them to the Big Five factors and performance dimensions (see also Barrick, Stewart, & Piotrowski, 2002; Caligiuri, 2000; Clevenger, Pereira, Wiechmann, Schmitt, & Schmidt Harvey, 2001). The third column of Table 1 indicates which scales of the OPQ were selected. For instance, Extraversion was measured with the Affiliative and Outgoing subscales of the OPQ. We used the Concept 4.2 version of the OPQ. In total, this version of the OPQ consisted of 360 items measuring 31 scales. Candidates were required to select one least and one most preferred item from a set of four alternatives. They completed the OPQ in their own language. Because the OPQ is the property of the publisher, we received only candidates’ scores on the subscales. Hence, we were not able to compute the reliability of the scales. However, prior studies that conducted analyses at the item level have found satisfactory reliabilities (e.g., Matthews et al., 1990). For the scales included in our study, test–retest reliabilities varied between .73 and .94, and internal consistencies varied between .68 and .89 (Matthews et al., 1990). Assessment center exercises. In the first exercise, an analysis– presentation exercise, each candidate assumed the role of a consultant and analyzed a complex set of facts and figures relating to various departments of a medium-sized organization. Each candidate had to determine and present the strategy for the next 5 years. This exercise provided opportunities to observe and evaluate the dimensions of tenacity–resilience, communication, adaptability, and organizational and commercial awareness (see Table 1). The second exercise was a group discussion in which candidates had to reach consensus on cost reductions. This exercise measured the dimensions of communication, adaptability, teamwork, and organizational and commercial awareness (see Table 1). Assessors were both experienced psychologists and ETP staff members. The psychologists came from a pool of 10 external consultants (7 women and 3 men; average assessor experience ⫽ 10 years, SD ⫽ 7 years). The rationale for including ETP staff was that they were knowledgeable about Japanese culture. Hence, they could give advice as to which cultural model to apply when deciding on the effectiveness of behaviors (Briscoe, 1997). Both psychologists and ETP staff had attended a 3-day training seminar in accordance with the Guidelines and Ethical Considerations for Assessment Center Operations (Task Force on Assessment Center Guidelines, 1989). Assessors were given behavioral checklists to aid their observation, recording, and classification tasks. After each exercise, they rated candidates on a 9-point rating scale ranging from poor (1) to outstanding (9) per

482

LIEVENS, HARRIS, VAN KEER, AND BISQUERET

dimension. There was one assessor per candidate. Candidates were rated by different assessors across exercises. Behavior description interview. Candidates were asked to give specific examples of past situations wherein they had demonstrated particular skills that were expected to be related to success in cross-situational training. The dimensions measured were self-discipline, tenacity–resilience, teamwork, and cross-cultural awareness (see Table 1). Only experienced psychologists served as interviewers. There was one interviewer per candidate. Interviewers were given a detailed interview guide, which listed example questions and answers per dimension. A sample question asked candidates to discuss a stressful situation they had to deal with in the past (related to the tenacity–resilience dimension). Probes included such inquiries as “What made the situation so stressful?,” “How did you handle the situation?,” “What else could you have done?,” and “What was the final result?” Candidates were rated on a 9-point rating scale ranging from poor (1) to outstanding (9) per dimension. On average, the interview lasted 45 min. Final decision. The ultimate decision as to whether to accept an applicant into the ETP was based on several ratings. First, candidates’ results on the aforementioned selection instruments were mechanically integrated into a final score. Second, the ETP staff conducted an interview wherein they asked applicants about the readiness and plans of their company for doing trade with Japan, the financial commitment of their company to the ETP, and related questions. Third, the external consultants and the ETP staff members clinically integrated candidates’ final selection procedure rating and ETP staff members’ judgments to determine the ETP fellows.

Criterion Measures Training performance. We obtained instructors’ ratings on the performance dimensions (see our description of the ETP and Table 1) that captured the business-related component of the ETP. Thirty instructors from the school at which the language courses and business-related seminars and workshops were held rated the managers after the managers had left the school (i.e., after 12 months). These instructors were involved with the managers on a daily basis. None of these instructors was familiar with the managers’ evaluation at the time of the selection in Europe. It was made clear that the data gathered would be kept confidential and for research purposes only. Instructors were required to evaluate a manager on the performance dimensions using a 5-point rating scale, ranging from poor (1) to outstanding (5). Behavioral examples were listed per performance dimension. The first column of Table 1 presents these performance dimensions and their descriptions. The median correlation among the performance dimension ratings was .39. Therefore, we computed a composite measure of general cross-cultural training performance. The internal consistency of this composite was .79. Language acquisition. As a second criterion, we obtained objective data about the language component of the cross-cultural training. These data captured the information acquisition component that is typical for cross-cultural training (Bhagat & Prien, 1996). Managers’ proficiency of the Japanese language was measured at the halfway point (6 months) and at the end of the courses (12 months). Each exam consisted of various subsections (e.g., reading, understanding, writing, and speaking Japanese). The total score for each exam was obtained by summing the scores on each subsection. The maximum total score on each exam was 200. Mean exam scores at 12 months (M ⫽ 127.81) were significantly higher than mean exam scores at 6 months (M ⫽ 76.32), t(75) ⫽ 32.01, p ⬍ .001 (d ⫽ 1.91). These two exam scores correlated .91. These two training performance criteria are independent because the language exams were conducted by an external organization and the instructors who gave the ratings on the dimensions were not familiar with the exam scores. In addition, the correlation between the final exam score and the instructors’ ratings was relatively low (r ⫽ .26, p ⬍ .05), indicating that these two criteria were empirically distinct.

Because only 86 of the 166 managers (52%) were selected for the ETP in Japan, 6 managers did not attend the ETP (although they were selected), and 2 managers left the ETP, training performance data were available for 78 managers.

Results Validity of Predictors Table 2 presents the means, standard deviations, and correlations among this study’s variables. The last two rows display the bivariate correlations between the various predictors and the criteria. We corrected correlations for direct range restriction (Thorndike, 1949) and for unreliability in the criterion. To correct for unreliability in instructor ratings of training performance, we used the same value (.80) as McDaniel et al. (1994). To correct for unreliability in the language proficiency criterion, we used the correlation between the two exams (.91). Statistical significance was determined prior to correcting the correlations (Sackett & Yang, 2000). Hypothesis 1 proposed that cognitive ability would be significantly related to cross-cultural training performance. In support of this hypothesis, cognitive ability was significantly correlated with the test measuring language acquisition (r ⫽ .23, p ⬍ .05; corrected r ⫽ .27). However, cognitive ability was not significantly correlated with instructors’ ratings of training performance (r ⫽ .09, ns; corrected r ⫽ .11). Thus, there was partial support for Hypothesis 1. The next set of hypotheses proposed significant relationships between cross-cultural training performance and Openness (Hypothesis 2a), Extraversion (Hypothesis 2b), and Conscientiousness (Hypothesis 2c). There was mixed support for this set of hypotheses. On the positive side, Openness was significantly correlated with instructors’ ratings of cross-cultural training performance (r ⫽ .31, p ⬍ .01; corrected r ⫽ .33). Yet Extraversion was not related to instructors’ ratings of training performance (r ⫽ ⫺.03, ns; corrected r ⫽ ⫺.04), nor was Conscientiousness (r ⫽ .18, ns; corrected r ⫽ .20). Although we did not have specific hypotheses about Emotional Stability or Agreeableness, we examined their correlations with instructors’ ratings of training performance. Emotional Stability was not significantly related to instructor ratings (r ⫽ ⫺.03, ns; corrected r ⫽ ⫺.04). Agreeableness was significantly, albeit negatively, related to instructor ratings (r ⫽ ⫺.24, p ⬍ .05; corrected r ⫽ ⫺.26). Emotional Stability was the only personality factor that was significantly, albeit negatively, correlated with the language proficiency test (r ⫽ ⫺.28, p ⬍ .05; corrected r ⫽ ⫺.29). Hypothesis 3 assumed that the dimensions as measured by assessment center exercises would be significantly related to crosscultural training performance. Partial support was obtained for this hypothesis. All dimensions measured in the group discussion exercise were significantly correlated with instructor ratings and with language proficiency. Teamwork, communication, adaptability, and organizational and commercial awareness as measured in the group discussion exercise had uncorrected correlations varying from .25 ( p ⬍ .05) to .31 ( p ⬍ .01) with instructor ratings (range of corrected rs ⫽ .31 to .40) and from .28 ( p ⬍ .05) to .38 ( p ⬍ .01) with the language proficiency test (range of corrected rs ⫽ .33 to .44). However, none of the dimensions measured in the analysis–presentation exercise was significantly correlated with

Cognitive ability Emotional stability Extraversion Openness Agreeableness Conscientiousness Self-discipline a. Interview Tenacity–resilience a. Analysis–presentation b. Interview Teamwork a. Group discussion b. Interview Communication a. Group discussion b. Analysis–presentation Adaptability a. Group discussion b. Analysis–presentation Cross-cultural awareness a. Interview Organizational and commercial awareness a. Group discussion b. Analysis–presentation 0.53

1.79 1.78

1.37

1.77 1.74

1.66 1.72

1.76 1.25

1.55 1.21

127.81 30.53

3.35

3.95 4.61

6.05

4.69 5.01

5.33 5.61

5.35 5.80

5.60 6.09

.09 (.11) .23 (.27)

.25 .22

.03

.29 .25

.24 .14

.24 .20

.09 .11

.10

1.29

6.31

1

1.64 — 1.56 .02 1.57 .01 1.45 .08 2.07 ⫺.12 1.40 ⫺.03

SD

5.28 5.61 6.02 5.82 6.10 5.71

M

5

.01 .06

.13 .01

.08 .07

.13 .01

.03 ⫺.05

.08 .01

⫺.03 ⫺.07

⫺.02 ⫺.14

⫺.12 ⫺.17

⫺.04 .13

Criteria .31 ⫺.24 .18 (.33) (⫺.26) (.20) .10 .03 .12 (.10) (.03) (.12)

.05 ⫺.01 .12 .04

.13 ⫺.08

.09 .16

.08 .05 .14 ⫺.01

.00 .00

.19



⫺.09 .02 ⫺.03 ⫺.01

⫺.02

⫺.02 ⫺.23 .14 ⫺.01 ⫺.06 .00

— ⫺.05

.10 .09

6



7a

.36 .67

.23

.38 .77

.31 .81

.26 .20

— .23

8a

.13 .21

.27

.15 .22

.08 .26

.10 .36



8b

.12 .10 ⫺.04 (.15) (.15) (⫺.05) .01 ⫺.09 ⫺.12 (.01) (⫺.12) (⫺.14)

.23 .20

.36

.27 .20

.21 .16

.31 .36

.18 .41

Predictors (N ⫽ 166)

4

— ⫺.06 — .05 ⫺.30 ⫺.22 ⫺.13

3

⫺.03 ⫺.03 (⫺.04) (⫺.04) ⫺.28 .07 (⫺.29) (.08)

.04 ⫺.05

⫺.11

.01 .00

⫺.02 .04

⫺.01 .00

.05 ⫺.02

.01

— ⫺.23 ⫺.03 ⫺.07 ⫺.03

2

.17 .26

.42

.21 .26

.19 .24



9b

.76 .47

.22

.83 .42

— .35

10a

.37 .69

.24

.41 .76



10b

.88 .54

.27

— .48

11a

.44 .84

.26



11b

.25 .22



.25 (.31) .28 (.33)

— .51

.13 — (.17) .10 .26 — (.12) — —



12a 13a 13b 14 15

.30 .15 .31 .06 .28 .11 .05 (.38) (.18) (.40) (.08) (.37) (.15) (.07) .38 ⫺.01 .35 ⫺.10 .33 ⫺.02 .14 (.44) (⫺.01) (.41) (⫺.12) (.41) (⫺.02) (.18)

.77 .45

.28

.88 .37

.88 .33

— .31

9a

Note. Correlations between predictors and training performance that were corrected for range restriction and criterion unreliability appear in parentheses. Statistical significance was determined prior to the range restriction correction. Uncorrected correlations with the criteria of training performance and language acquisition above .22 are significant at p ⬍ .05 and above .29 at p ⬍ .01. Other uncorrected correlations above .16 are significant at p ⬍ .05 and above .20 at p ⬍ .01.

14. Training performance (N ⫽ 78) 15. Language proficiency (N ⫽ 78)

13.

12.

11.

10.

9.

8.

1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7.

Variable

Table 2 Means, Standard Deviations, and Correlations of Study Variables

PREDICTORS OF CROSS-CULTURAL TRAINING PERFORMANCE

483

484

LIEVENS, HARRIS, VAN KEER, AND BISQUERET

the criteria (range of uncorrected rs ⫽ ⫺.04 to .13 for training performance and ⫺.12 to .10 for language acquisition). Hypothesis 4 proposed that there would be a significant relationship between dimensions measured by the behavior description interview and cross-cultural training performance. No support was found for this hypothesis because none of the dimensions measured by the behavior description interview yielded significant correlations with our criteria.

Incremental Validity Hypothesis 5 posited that dimensions measured by assessment center exercises and a behavior description interview would have incremental validity over personality and cognitive ability. Our general strategy for testing this hypothesis consisted of holding the construct under investigation constant. Accordingly, our analyses avoided the pitfall of confounding the content (constructs measured) and the methods (techniques used to measure the specific content) (Arthur et al., in press; Chan & Schmitt, 1997; Schmitt & Chan, 1998; Schmitt & Mills, 2001). To test Hypothesis 5, for instance, we conducted a hierarchical regression analysis in which we entered one Big Five personality factor (e.g., Openness) and cognitive ability in the first step. In the second step, we entered a conceptually related dimension (e.g., Adaptability) as measured by an analysis–presentation exercise and a group discussion exercise. Hence, we could examine whether Adaptability as measured by an analysis–presentation exercise and a group discussion exercise added additional variance over cognitive ability and the conceptually related personality trait of Openness. We linked Adaptability to Openness because prior research showed that these were related constructs (LePine et al., 2000). In another hierarchical regression analysis, we investigated whether teamwork as measured by a group discussion and a behavior description interview explained additional variance, even when cognitive ability and Agreeableness were already accounted for. We linked teamwork to Agreeableness because prior research demonstrated that these were related constructs (Mount, Barrick, & Stewart, 1998). Table 1 shows which three other conceptually relevant linkages were examined to test Hypothesis 5. Because multivariate range restriction and criterion unreliability might also affect the regression results, we applied the appropriate corrections (see Ree et al., 1995) to the matrix of correlations (Table 2) and used this corrected matrix as input for the hierarchical regression analyses. Statistical significance was determined prior to applying the corrections (by conducting hierarchical regressions on the uncorrected matrix of correlations). Table 3 presents the results of these hierarchical regression analyses for both of our training performance criteria. Hypothesis 5 was partially confirmed because three dimensions accounted for a significant amount of additional variance in cross-cultural training performance. Teamwork significantly explained additional variance in our two criteria of training performance over and beyond cognitive ability and Agreeableness. Communication accounted for a significant additional portion of the variance in training performance over and beyond cognitive ability and Extraversion. Adaptability added a significant amount of variance over cognitive ability and Openness. The dimensions of selfconfidence and tenacity–resilience did not provide incremental

validity over cognitive ability and conceptually related personality traits.

Discussion In this study we examined the validity of a broad set of predictors for selecting European managers for a cross-cultural training program in Japan. The selection procedure assessed personality traits and cognitive ability and also dimensions measured by assessment center exercises and a behavior description interview. This study has several important contributions with regard to the validity of these predictors in an international context, their incremental validity, and the criterion (cross-cultural training performance) used to evaluate them.

Validity of Individual Predictors in an International Context Although there exists a large research base about predictors in a traditional domestic selection context (Schmidt & Hunter, 1998), relatively few studies have examined these predictors in an international context. This is especially the case for predictors of cross-cultural training, despite the growing practical and scientific importance of cross-cultural training. Therefore, one of the contributions of this study consists in demonstrating which validity results obtained in a domestic context hold in an international selection context. Many, but certainly not all, of our hypotheses for the validity results were supported. A traditionally valid predictor as cognitive ability was significantly related to the language acquisition dimension of cross-cultural training performance, but it was not significantly related to other aspects of cross-cultural training performance. With regard to the personality traits, Openness was significantly related to instructors’ ratings of cross-cultural training performance. In line with our hypothesis, this result extends the training findings for Openness to an international context. It also fits well with the general contention that open individuals are more adaptable and therefore more ready to accept differences between various cultures (Bhagat & Prien, 1996; Bing & Lounsbury, 2000). Conversely, no support was found for our hypotheses that Extraversion and Conscientiousness would be significantly related to cross-cultural training success. A closer look at our correlation between Conscientiousness and cross-cultural training (r ⫽ .18; corrected r ⫽ .20), nevertheless, indicates that it is quite close to the meta-analytic value (r ⫽ .13; corrected r ⫽ .23) reported by Barrick and Mount (1991). Thus, although our correlation between Conscientiousness and instructor ratings did not reach statistical significance here, it is approximately the same as that reported by Barrick and Mount for overall training proficiency. Contrary to the extant literature, some validity coefficients for the Big Five factors (e.g., Agreeableness) were negative. On the one hand, these divergent results may stem from the use of an ipsative personality measure (Baron, 1996; Hicks, 1970). On the other hand, as argued by Tett, Jackson, Rothstein, and Reddon (1999), these negative correlations may reflect true relationships between the predictors and the criteria (e.g., for an explanation for the negative correlation of Agreeableness in an international context, see Caligiuri, 2000), which raises the possibility that person-

PREDICTORS OF CROSS-CULTURAL TRAINING PERFORMANCE

485

Table 3 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analyses of Cross-Cultural Training Performance and Language Proficiency on Predictors Criterion Training performance Model 1 2

1 2

1 2

1 2

1 2

Predictors



Cognitive ability Emotional Stability Tenacity (analysis–presentation) Tenacity (interview)

.18 .08 .20 ⫺.05

Cognitive ability Extraversion Communication (group discussion) Communication (analysis–presentation)

.08 ⫺.04 .43** .10

Cognitive ability Openness Adaptability (analysis–presentation) Adaptability (group discussion)

Cognitive ability Agreeableness Teamwork (interview) Teamwork (group discussion)

Cognitive ability Conscientiousness Self-discipline

.05 .32** .11 .34*

.04 ⫺.28* .15 .41*

.18 .12 .16

R2

Language acquisition ⌬R2

.04

.30 ⫺.26* ⫺.12 ⫺.13

.21*

.20 .10 .47** ⫺.30

.15

.20 .10 ⫺.29 .48**

.21*

.23* .16 ⫺.26 .46**

.02

.27* .06 .02

.05 .08 Adj. R2 ⫽ .03 Cross-validity ⫽ .02 .04 .25 Adj. R2 ⫽ .20 Cross-validity ⫽ .18 .14* .29 Adj. R2 ⫽ .25 Cross-validity ⫽ .22 .10 .31 Adj. R2 ⫽ .27 Cross-validity ⫽ .25 .06 .08 Adj. R2 ⫽ .04 Cross-validity ⫽ .03



R2

⌬R2

.15** .18 Adj. R2 ⫽ .14 Cross-validity ⫽ .11

.04

.08 .25 Adj. R2 ⫽ .25 Cross-validity ⫽ .22

.16**

.08 .26 Adj. R2 ⫽ .21 Cross-validity ⫽ .19

.17**

.09 .28 Adj. R2 ⫽ .24 Cross-validity ⫽ .22 .08 .08 Adj. R2 ⫽ .04 Cross-validity ⫽ .03

.20**

.00

Note. N ⫽ 77. Estimates are for second step, not entry. Input correlation matrix was corrected for multivariate range restriction and criterion unreliability. Cross-validity was computed with the formula of Cattin (1980). Because of rounding, ⌬R2 differs .01 from cumulative R2. Adj. ⫽ adjusted. * p ⬍ .05. ** p ⬍ .01.

ality scores may be interpreted in the wrong direction if these scales are used to make decisions about entry into a cross-cultural training program. In any case, there is a clear need to replicate our findings regarding personality with other measures and in other international contexts. In partial support of our hypothesis with regard to the assessment center dimensions, communication, adaptability, and teamwork as measured in the group discussion were significantly correlated with the instructors’ training performance ratings and with the language acquisition scores. The corrected validities obtained for these dimensions are even higher than the corrected mean validity (.35) of assessment centers for predicting training performance in the meta-analysis of Gaugler et al. (1987). When these dimensions were measured in the analysis–presentation, they were not significantly correlated with training performance. The most straightforward explanation for these results is that these dimensions closely mirrored the skills needed in the smallgroup learning setting of the ETP. In other words, executives who scored well on these dimensions (communication, adaptability,

and teamwork) in the group discussion also received high ratings from the instructors on the dimensions and were better able to study the Japanese language in small groups. In addition, assessors told us that the group exercise provided them with ample opportunities for evaluating each candidate’s quality of contribution to the team. Perhaps the other exercise (analysis–presentation) did not provide enough opportunities for observing and evaluating these dimensions. Apparently, to obtain good predictive validity, practitioners should pay attention to simulate the specific (cultural) environment that international managers will work in. Future studies are needed to single out which other assessment center design factors are of key importance to guarantee valid predictions in an international context (see Briscoe, 1997). For instance, should assessors come from both the sending and the receiving country? And should assessees with different cultural backgrounds be assessed together? Finally, we found no support for our hypothesis regarding the validity of high-structure interviews. Given the specified dimensions that it was designed to measure, the behavior description

486

LIEVENS, HARRIS, VAN KEER, AND BISQUERET

interview was not a significant predictor for our criteria. Perhaps a different behavior description interview designed to measure different but conceptually relevant dimensions could obtain significant criterion-related validities for these dimensions. Future research should also address whether a situational interview might work better. We suspect that in selecting for a training program or for a job in a foreign environment, an interview that focuses on how an applicant would perform on a dimension may be more valid than assessing past behavior in a general work setting. Research has just begun to explore which question format may be more effective in specific contexts (Huffcutt et al., 2001; Pulakos & Schmitt, 1995).

Incremental Validity in an International Context In recent years, a growing amount of studies have examined the incremental validity of multidimensional predictors such as assessment centers (Goffin et al., 1996), high-structure interviews (Cortina et al., 2000), situational judgment tests (Clevenger et al., 2001), and biodata (Mount, Witt, & Barrick, 2000) over cognitive ability and the Big Five personality traits. Our study contributed to this growing literature by examining incremental validity issues in an international context. An important finding was that only the dimensions of communication, teamwork, and adaptability as measured by a group discussion exercise provided incremental variance in cross-cultural training performance beyond cognitive ability and conceptually related personality traits. Another contribution to the developing incremental validity literature in personnel selection is that we used a construct-driven analytical strategy for examining incremental validity. When prior research found that assessment centers provided incremental variance beyond personality (Goffin et al., 1996), no meaningful conclusions could be drawn, as this result might have been found because assessment centers have a different selection procedure than a personality inventory or because assessment centers capture constructs other than the Big Five personality factors. In our analyses, however, the construct under investigation was held constant so that we were able to conclude that dimensions as measured by the assessment center method provided incremental variance over and above conceptually related constructs measured by a different selection method (i.e., a personality inventory). More generally, we hope that this study encourages researchers to adopt a construct-driven focus in incremental validity investigations.

Cross-Cultural Training Performance as Criterion This study focused on predictors of cross-cultural training performance. Hereby we assumed that if people did not succeed in the cross-cultural training in the first place, it would be unlikely that they would perform successfully in the cross-cultural assignment (see Caligiuri, 2000). To examine whether the ETP also predicted job performance, we gathered data regarding executives’ performance in Japanese companies. As mentioned above, the European executives worked in Japanese companies after the ETP. We asked the Japanese supervisors of the executives to rate them after 6 months on similar rating forms, as described in the Method section. These supervisors were familiar neither with the executives’ ETP evaluation nor with their selection evaluation in Europe.

Rating forms were returned for 33 of the 78 managers. Both criteria of cross-cultural training performance were significantly correlated with a composite measure (␣ ⫽ .89) of executives’ performance in Japanese companies (r ⫽ .38, p ⬍ .05, for instructors’ ratings and r ⫽ .45, p ⬍ .05, for language proficiency). These correlations are in the same range as the meta-analytic correlations found by Deshpande and Viswesvaran (1992) and demonstrate that ETP performance is related to subsequent job performance in Japan.1 Similar to Caligiuri (2000), this study used a process of selecting people into cross-cultural training, providing those selected with cross-cultural training, and then sending abroad those who passed the training. An important advantage of this process is that it may reduce the costs associated with international assignments because only people who have passed the selection and therefore are considered to be predisposed for expatriate success are sent to the training and abroad. Similar cost arguments have been made in the context of selecting trainees for traditional training (I. L. Goldstein & Ford, 2002). In addition, because the selection and training procedures might serve as mini–work samples for future expatriate performance, they might shape realistic perceptions about the foreign culture. Besides this model of selection, training, and assignment, another process simply consists of selecting people for the international assignment and sending those selected abroad. Research is needed to compare the viability of these and other international human resources staffing models.

Limitations of Study This study has several limitations. First, this study involved European managers participating in cross-cultural training. Whether our results are due to the use of non–North American managers or whether they are due to success in a cross-cultural training program as the criterion needs to be determined through future research. So far, meta-analytic validity results for personality (Salgado, 1997) and cognitive ability (Salgado & Anderson, 2002) that were obtained in Europe have shown good correspondence with North American results. Second, as with most studies with international managers (Arthur & Bennett, 1997), this study had a relatively small sample size. As a result, the statistical power to detect significant relationships was low (although we found many significant results nevertheless). Because of these limitations, our findings should be interpreted with caution, and studies with other and larger samples and different criterion measures are needed to confirm our findings. Third, even though a variety of predictors were used for selecting people into a cross-cultural training program, some potentially 1

Because language acquisition scores significantly improved during the ETP (see Method section), we have evidence that the significant relationship between language acquisition scores and job performance in Japanese companies was due to the language training component of the ETP. However, as noted by an anonymous reviewer, because no data were available showing that the executives improved on the performance dimensions during the ETP, the significant correlation between instructors’ ratings of training performance and supervisors’ ratings of job performance might be due to factors (e.g., the fact that both instructors and supervisors were Japanese) other than the skill training received during the ETP.

PREDICTORS OF CROSS-CULTURAL TRAINING PERFORMANCE

interesting predictors were not included (see Colquitt et al., 2000; I. L. Goldstein & Ford, 2002). An example is self-efficacy. In the domestic context, self-efficacy predicts training performance, and training programs built to modify self-efficacy also appear to be effective in changing job performance (Ford, Quinones, Sego, & Sorra, 1992).

Summary The selection of international managers is a field full of opportunities and challenges for both practitioners and researchers. The selection procedure in this study broadened existing expatriate selection procedures by incorporating cognitive ability, personality, and dimensions measured by assessment center exercises and a behavior description interview. The overall results are encouraging because the validity coefficients are relatively consistent with the existing literature in the domestic context, as is the incremental validity for assessment center dimensions, although some of the validity coefficients operated differently than expected.

References Arthur, W., Jr., & Bennett, W., Jr. (1997). A comparative test of alternative models of international assignee job performance. In D. M. Saunders (Series Ed.) & Z. Aycan (Vol. Ed.), New approaches to employee management: Vol. 4. Expatriate management: Theory and research (pp. 141–172). Stamford, CT: JAI Press. Arthur, W., Jr., Day, E. A., McNelly, T. L., & Edens, P. S. (in press). A meta-analysis of the criterion-related validity of assessment center dimensions. Personnel Psychology. Arvey, R. D., Bhagat, R. S., & Salas, E. (1991). Cross-cultural and cross-national issues in personnel and human resources management: Where do we go from here? In G. R. Ferrris & K. M. Rowland (Eds.), Research in personnel and human resource management (Vol. 9, pp. 367– 407). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Aryee, S. (1997). Selection and training of expatriate employees. In N. Anderson & P. Herriot (Eds.), International handbook of selection and assessment (pp. 147–160). Chichester, UK: Wiley. Baron, H. (1996). Strengths and limitations of ipsative measurement. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 69, 49 –56. Barrick, M. R., & Mount, M. K. (1991). The Big Five personality dimensions and job performance: A meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology, 44, 1–26. Barrick, M. R., Stewart, G. L., & Piotrowski, M. (2002). Personality and job performance: Test of the mediating effects of motivation among sales representatives. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 43–51. Beaujouan, Y. (2000). The convergence of the factorial structure of the OPQ (Occupational Personality Questionnaire) towards the five factor model, in seven countries. European Review of Applied Psychology, 50, 349 –357. Bhagat, R. S., & Prien, K. O. (1996). Cross-cultural training in organizational contexts. In D. Landis & R. S. Bhagat (Eds.), Handbook of intercultural training (pp. 216 –230). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Bhawuk, D. P. S. (1998). The role of culture theory in cross-cultural training: A multimethod study of culture-specific, culture-general, and culture theory-based assimilators. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 29, 630 – 655. Bhawuk, D. P. S., & Brislin, R. W. (2000). Cross-cultural training: A review. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 49, 162–191. Bing, M. N., & Lounsbury, J. W. (2000). Openness and job performance in U.S.-based Japanese manufacturing companies. Journal of Business and Psychology, 14, 515–522.

487

Black, J. S., & Gregersen, H. B. (1991). Antecedents to cross-cultural adjustment for expatriates in Pacific Rim assignments. Human Relations, 44, 497–515. Black, J. S., & Mendenhall, M. (1989). A practical but theory-based framework for selecting cross-cultural training methods. Human Resource Management, 28, 511–539. Black, J. S., & Mendenhall, M. (1990). Cross-cultural training effectiveness: A review and a theoretical framework for future research. Academy of Management Review, 15, 113–136. Blickle, G. (1996). Personality traits, learning strategies, and performance. European Journal of Personality, 10, 337–352. Briscoe, D. R. (1997). Assessment centers: Cross-cultural and crossnational issues. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 12, 261– 270. Caligiuri, P. M. (2000). The Big Five personality characteristics as predictors of expatriate’s desire to terminate the assignment and supervisorrated performance. Personnel Psychology, 53, 67– 88. Campion, M. A., Pursell, E. D., & Brown, B. K. (1988). Structured interviewing: Raising the psychometric properties of the employment interview. Personnel Psychology, 41, 25– 42. Cattin, P. (1980). Estimation of a regression model. Journal of Applied Psychology, 65, 407– 414. Chan, D., & Schmitt, N. (1997). Video-based versus paper-and-pencil method of assessment in situational judgment tests: Subgroup differences in test performance and face validity perceptions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 143–159. Church, A. T. (1982). Sojourner adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 91, 540 –572. Clevenger, J., Pereira, G. M., Wiechmann, D., Schmitt, N., & Schmidt Harvey, V. (2001). Incremental validity of situational judgment tests. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 410 – 417. Colquitt, J., LePine, J. A., & Noe, R. A. (2000). Toward an integrative theory of training motivation: A meta-analytic path analysis of 20 years of research. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 654 – 665. Colquitt, J., & Simmering, M. J. (1998). Conscientiousness, goal orientation, and motivation to learn during the learning process: A longitudinal study. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 678 –707. Cortina, J. M., Goldstein, N. B., Payne, S. C., Davison, H. K., & Gilliland, S. W. (2000). The incremental validity of interview scores over and above cognitive ability and conscientiousness scores. Personnel Psychology, 53, 325–351. Dalton, M., & Wilson, M. (2000). The relationship of the five-factor model of personality to job performance for a group of Middle Eastern expatriate managers. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 18, 250 –258. Deller, J. (1997). Expatriate selection: Possibilities and limitations of using personality scales. In D. M. Saunders (Series Ed.) & Z. Aycan (Vol. Ed.), Expatriate management: Theory and research (pp. 93–116). Stamford, CT: JAI Press. Deshpande, S. P., & Viswesvaran, C. (1992). Is cross-cultural training of expatriate managers effective? A meta analysis. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 16, 295–310. Dowling, P. J., Welch, D. E., & Schuler, R. S. (1999). International human resource management. Cincinnati, OH: South-Western. Duane, M. (2001). Policies and practices in global human resource systems. Westport, CT: Quorum. Earley, P. C. (1987). Intercultural training for managers: A comparison of documentary and interpersonal methods. Academy of Management Journal, 30, 685– 698. Ferguson, E., Payne, T., & Anderson, N. (1994). Occupational personality assessment: Theory, structure and psychometrics of the OPQ FMX5– Student. Personality and Individual Differences, 17, 217–225. Ford, J. K., Quinones, M. A., Sego, D. J., & Sorra, J. S. (1992). Factors affecting the opportunity to perform trained tasks on the job. Personnel Psychology, 45, 511–527.

488

LIEVENS, HARRIS, VAN KEER, AND BISQUERET

Gaugler, B. B., Rosenthal, D. B., Thornton, G. C., & Bentson, C. (1987). Meta-analysis of assessment center validity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 72, 493–511. Goffin, R. D., Rothstein, M. G., & Johnston, N. G. (1996). Personality testing and the assessment center: Incremental validity for managerial selection. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81, 746 –756. Goldstein, H. W., Yusko, K. P., Braverman, E. P., Smith, D. B., & Chung, B. (1998). The role of cognitive ability in the subgroup differences and incremental validity of assessment center exercises. Personnel Psychology, 51, 357–374. Goldstein, I. L., & Ford, J. K. (2002) Training in organizations: Needs assessment, development, and evaluation (4th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth/Thomson Learning. Hammer, M. R., & Martin, J. N. (1992). The effects of cross-cultural training on American managers in a Japanese-American joint venture. Journal of Applied Communication Research, 20, 161–182. Harrison, J. K. (1992). Individual and combined effects of behavior modeling and the cultural assimilator in the cross-cultural management training. Journal of Applied Psychology, 77, 952–962. Hicks, L. E. (1970). Some properties of ipsative, normative, and forcedchoice normative measures. Psychological Bulletin, 74, 167–184. Hough, L. M., Eaton, N. K., Dunnette, M. D., Kamp, J. D., & McCloy, R. A. (1990). Criterion-related validities of personality constructs and the effect of response distortion on those validities. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75, 581–595. Hough, L. M., & Oswald, F. L. (2000). Personnel selection: Looking toward the future—Remembering the past. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 631– 664. Huffcutt, A., Roth, P., & McDaniel, M. (1996). A meta-analytic investigation of cognitive ability in employment interview evaluations: Moderating characteristics and implications for incremental validity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81, 459 – 473. Huffcutt, A. I., & Arthur, W., Jr. (1994). Hunter and Hunter (1984) revisited: Interview validity for entry-level jobs. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 184 –190. Huffcutt, A. I., Conway, J. M., Roth, P. L., & Stone, N. J. (2001). Identification and meta-analytic assessment of psychological constructs measured in employment interviews. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 897–913. Huffcutt, A. I., Weekley, J., Wiesner, W. H., DeGroot, T. G., & Jones, C. (2001). Comparison of situational and behavior description interview questions for higher-level positions. Personnel Psychology, 54, 619 – 644. Hunter, J. E. (1986). Cognitive ability, cognitive aptitude, job knowledge, and job performance. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 29, 340 –362. Hurtz, G. M., & Donovan, J. J. (2000). Personality and job performance: The Big Five revisited. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 869 – 879. Janz, J. T. (1982). Initial comparisons of patterned behavior description interviews versus unstructured interviews. Journal of Applied Psychology, 67, 577–580. Kanfer, R., & Ackerman, P. L. (1989). Motivation and cognitive abilities: An integrative/aptitude-treatment interaction approach to skill acquisition. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 657– 690. Latham, G. P., Saari, L. M., Pursell, E. D., & Campion, M. A. (1980). The situational interview. Journal of Applied Psychology, 65, 422– 427. LePine, J. A., Colquitt, J. A., & Erez, A. (2000). Adaptability to changing task contexts: Effects of general cognitive ability, Conscientiousness, and Openness to Experience. Personnel Psychology, 53, 563–593. Love, K. G., Bishop, R. C., Heinisch, D. A., & Montei, M. S. (1994). Selection across two cultures: Adapting the selection of American assemblers to meet Japanese job performance demands. Personnel Psychology, 47, 837– 846. Matthews, G., & Stanton, N. (1994). Item and scale factor analyses of the

Occupational Personality Questionnaire. Personality and Individual Differences, 16, 733–743. Matthews, G., Stanton, N., Graham, N. C., & Brimelow, C. (1990). A factor analysis of the scales of the Occupational Personality Questionnaire. Personality and Individual Differences, 11, 591–596. McDaniel, M. A., Whetzel, D. L., Schmidt, F. L., & Maurer, S. D. (1994). The validity of employment interviews: A comprehensive review and meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 599 – 616. Mendenhall, M., & Oddou, G. (1985). The dimensions of expatriate acculturation. Academy of Management Review, 10, 39 – 48. Mendenhall, M., & Stahl, G. K. (2000). Expatriate training and development: Where do we go from here? Human Resource Management, 39, 251–265. Mount, M. K., Barrick, M. R., & Stewart, G. L. (1998). Five-factor model of personality and performance in jobs involving interpersonal interactions. Human Performance, 11, 145–165. Mount, M. K., Witt, L. A., & Barrick, M. R. (2000). Incremental validity of empirically keyed biodata scales over GMA and the five factor personality constructs. Personnel Psychology, 53, 299 –323. Olea, M. M., & Ree, M. J. (1994). Predicting pilot and navigator criteria: Not much more than g. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 845– 851. Ones, D. S., & Viswesvaran, C. (1997). Personality determinants in the prediction of aspects of expatriate job success. In D. M. Saunders (Series Ed.) & Z. Aycan (Vol. Ed.), New approaches to employee management: Vol. 4. Expatriate management: Theory and research (pp. 63–92). Stamford, CT: JAI Press. Pulakos, E. D., & Schmitt, N. (1995). Experience-based and situational interview questions: Studies of validity. Personnel Psychology, 48, 289 –308. Pynes, J. E., & H. J. Bernardin. (1989). Predictive validity of an entry-level police officer assessment center. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 831– 833. Ree, M. J., Carretta, T. R., & Teachout, M. S. (1995). Role of ability and prior knowledge in complex training performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 80, 721–730. Ree, M. J., & Earles, J. A. (1991). Predicting training success: Not much more than g. Personnel Psychology, 44, 321–332. Robertson, I. T., & Downs, S. (1989). Work-sample tests of trainability: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 402– 410. Robertson, I. T., & Kinder, A. (1993). Personality and job competences: The criterion-related validity of some personality variables. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 66, 225–244. Ronen, S. (1989). Training the international assignee. In I. L. Goldstein (Ed.), Training and development in organizations (pp. 417– 453). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Sackett, P. R., & Yang, H. (2000). Correction for range restriction: An expanded typology. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 112–118. Salas, E., & Cannon-Bowers, J. A. (2001). The science of training: A decade of progress. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 471– 499. Salgado, J. F. (1997). The five-factor model of personality and job performance in the European Community. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 30 – 43. Salgado, J. F., & Anderson, N. R. (2002). Cognitive and GMA testing in the European Community: Issues and evidence. Human Performance, 15, 75–96. Saville & Holdsworth. (1989). Management and graduate item bank. Surrey, England: Author. Saville & Holdsworth. (1990). Occupational Personality Questionnaire Manual. Surrey, England: Author. Saville, P., Sik, G., Nyfield, G., Hackston, J., & MacIver, R. (1996). A demonstration of the Occupational Personality Questionnaire (OPQ) in the measurement of job competencies across time and in separate organisations. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 45, 243–263. Schmidt, F. L., & Hunter, J. E. (1998). The validity and utility of selection

PREDICTORS OF CROSS-CULTURAL TRAINING PERFORMANCE methods in personnel psychology: Practical and theoretical implications of 85 years of research findings. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 262–274. Schmit, M. J., Kihm, J. A., & Robie, C. (2000). Development of a global measure of personality. Personnel Psychology, 53, 153–193. Schmitt, N., & Chan, D. (1998). Personnel selection: A theoretical approach. Thousands Oaks, CA: Sage. Schmitt, N., & Mills, A. E. (2001). Traditional tests and job simulations: Minority and majority performance and test validities. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 451– 458. Sinangil, H. K., & Ones, D. S. (2001). Expatriate management. In N. Anderson, D. S. Ones, H. K. Sinangil, & C. Viswesvaran (Eds.), Handbook of industrial, work & organizational psychology: Vol. 1. Personnel psychology (pp. 425– 443). London, UK: Sage. Spreitzer, G. M., McCall, M. W., & Mahoney, J. D. (1997). Early identification of international executive potential. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 6 –29. Stahl, G. K. (1995). Ein strukturiertes auswahlinterview fu¨ r den auslandseinsatz [Development of a structured interview for the selection of managers for an overseas assignment]. Zeitschrift fu¨r Arbeits- und Organisationspsychologie, 39, 84 –90. Task Force on Assessment Center Guidelines. (1989). Guidelines and

489

ethical considerations for assessment center operations. Public Personnel Management, 18, 457– 470. Tett, R. P., Jackson, D. N., & Rothstein, M. (1991). Personality measures as predictors of job performance: A meta-analytic review. Personnel Psychology, 44, 703–742. Tett, R. P., Jackson, D. N., Rothstein, M., & Reddon, J. R. (1999). Meta-analysis of bidirectional relations in personality–job performance research. Human Performance, 12, 1–29. Thorndike, R. L. (1949). Personnel selection: Test and measurement techniques. New York: Wiley. Tung, R. L. (1981). Selection and training of personnel for overseas assignments. Columbia Journal of World Business, 16, 68 –78. Wiesner, W. H., & Cronshaw, S. F. (1988). A meta-analytic investigation of the impact of interview format and degree of structure on the validity of the employment interview. Journal of Occupational Psychology, 61, 275–290.

Received February 27, 2002 Revision received December 13, 2002 Accepted December 19, 2002 䡲