PROBLEMS OF RELATIVE GROWTH

9 downloads 22186 Views 1MB Size Report
p. cm.-(Foundations of natural history). Originally published: New York: Dial Press, 1932. Includes bibliographical references (p. ) and indexes. 591.3'1-dc20.
PROBLEMS OF RELATIVE GROWTH Julian S. Huxley with a new introduction by Frederick B. Churchill and an essay by Richard E. Strauss

THE JOHNS HOPKINS UNIVERSITY PRESS Baltimore and London

This edition is reprinted by arrangement with the estate of Sir Julian S. Huxley. It has been reproduced from the 1932 Dial Press edition. © 1993 The Johns Hopkins University Press

All rights reserved. Published 1993 Printed in the United States of America on acid-free paper The Johns Hopkins University Press 2715 North Charles Street Baltimore, Maryland 21218-4319 The Johns Hopkins Press Ltd., London Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Huxley, Julian, 1887-1975. Problems of relative growth I Julian S. Huxley ; with a new introduction by Frederick B. Churchill and an essay by Richard E. Strauss. p. cm.-(Foundations of natural history) Originally published: New York: Dial Press, 1932. Includes bibliographical references (p. ) and indexes. ISBN 0-8018-4659-5 (pbk. : acid-free paper) 1. Allometry. I. Title. II. Series. QP84.H8 1993 92-43365 591.3'1-dc20 A catalog record for this book is available from the British Library.

THE STUDY OF ALLOMETRY SINCE HUXLEY Richard E. Strauss N looking back on Julian Huxley's work, and in particular on his emphasis on quantitative methods for the study of growth, it is easy to forget that the role of mathematics in biology remained a matter of controversy well into the twentieth century. Though mathematics had become an essential tool in physics and chemistry by the turn of the century, its acceptance by biologists was delayed for many years by a widely held philosophy of vitalism. Karl Pearson, the founder and first editor of the journal Biometrika, was both prophetic and optimistic when he said that "the day must come when the biologist will-without being a mathematician-not hesitate to use mathematical analysis when he requires it" (Pearson, 1901). But even as late as 1945, D'Arcy Thompson complained, "The zoologist or morphologist has been slow ... to invoke the aid of physical and mathematical sciences. . . . To treat the living body as a mechanism was repugnant, and seemed even ludicrous .... Even now the zoologist has scarce begun to dream of defining in mathematical language even the simplest organic forms .... In short, he is deeply reluctant to compare the living with the dead, or to explain by geometry or by mechanics the things which have their part in the mystery of life" (Thompson, 1945). It was in this context that quantitatively oriented biologists such as Huxley, Thompson, Minot, Robertson, Brody, and others developed and applied their early quantitative models of organismic size and shape. The shape of an organism is a fundamental feature of its overall design, and an obvious aspect of the shape of an individual is that it often changes markedly, and in a continuous manner, throughout its ontogeny. Huxley's fundamental deduction about body form was his "law" of the constant differential growth ratio (Huxley, 1924), his finding that "the ratio between the intensity (or relative rate) of growth of the organ and that of the body remains constant over long periods of the animal's life." From this observation followed the principles

I

xlviii

PROBLEMS OF RELATIVE GROWTH

and applications detailed in Problems of Relative Growth and his later publications. Huxley, however, was not the first to make, or even to quantify, the observation that morphological structures show a marked progressive change of relative size with increase in absolute size. As early as 1917, Thompson (1945) noted that all but the simplest organisms attain their adult form by differential growth in different directions. Pezard (1918, 1921) termed these changes heterogonic, while Champy (1924) described them as dysharmonic. Even Huxley's classical power function Y = b)(k (his heterogony formula, now known as the simple allometry formula) had been used earlier (apparently unknown to Huxley) by Snell (1891) and Dubois (1898, 1914, 1918) to determine the cephalization coefficient (ratio of brain weight to body weight) in mammals. Lapicque (1898) and Klatt (1919) applied the same techniques to birds, and Hecht (1913, 1916) and Crozier (1914; Crozier and Hecht, 1914) applied them to fishes. These earlier applications, however, were entirely empirical or were based merely on dimensional analysis (assuming isometry), and Huxley was the first to derive the expression from biological principles of growth and to propose their general application to the dimensions of animals and plants. Since Huxley's innovation of the allometry concept in the 1920s, a great deal of significant experimental and descriptive research has been added to it, so much more is now known about its physiological basis and general applicability (Brody, 1945; Le Gros Clark et al., 1945; Katz, 1980). Allometric equations are widely used in many biological disciplines to describe systematic changes in morphogenesis, physiology, adaptation, and evolution. Indeed, "allometry is perhaps the major principle regulating basic differences in form among closely related animals" (Gould, 197 5). THE BIOLOGICAL BASIS OF ALLOMETRY Early attitudes about Huxley's allometric model varied between two extremes: considering it to be a fundamental law of organic growth, of sufficient importance to potentially "explain the scale of being" (Quiring, 1941), and considering it to be merely a convenient statistical tool for summarizing data (Clark and Hersh, 1939). Current opinion about Huxley's model still varies, but not as widely. Although some biologists consider it to be a model of convenience (e.g., Smith, 1980), most recognize that it has at least a moderately firm basis in

STUDY OF ALLOMETRY SINCE HUXLEY

xlix

cellular patterns of differential growth (Katz, 1980; Gerhart et al., 1982; Atchley and Hall, 1991). Huxley's derivation of the bivariate allometric model in Problems of Relative Growth was based explicitly on the process of specific or multiplicative cellular growth (Lumar, 1937; Reeve and Huxley, 1945; Medawar, 1950; Laird, 1965; Perkkio and Keskinen, 1985). The allometric power function Y = b)(k can be shown to be the solution of the differential equation relating the specific growth rates of two mensural traits X and Y with respect to time (Turner, 1978; Lande, 1985; Shea, 1985): By · log Y

Bt

=

k BX · log X,

Bt

or

Thus, the allometric coefficient k is equal to the ratio of the specific growth rates of X and Y (Reeve and Huxley, 1945); that is, k is an index of the displacement in time of the growth process (or growth curve) of one trait in relation to that of the other trait (Laird et al., 1968). To the extent that the specific rates remain proportional to one another during growth, k remains constant. Nonproportional rates will produce nonlinear bivariate logarithmic plots (i.e., nonloglinear plots), indicating that the allometric coefficient is either a continuous function of age (Creighton and Strauss, 1986; Lebeau et al., 1986) or is multiphasic (Strauss and Fuiman, 1985; Kluge and Strauss, 1985). Huxley (1932) did note that there were several difficulties in applying his power function to studies of differential growth. Of the two most critical problems, he noted first that allometric coefficients are, at best, merely average values for large regions of the body form. If his concept of a growth gradient is really justified, we should expect a gradual and progressive change in allometric coefficients from point to point along the body. Identification of such gradients is primarily an operational problem, owing to the practical difficulty of finding a sufficient number of distinctive points within a body region among which to take measurements. The second principal difficulty, and one that is not widely

PROBLEMS OF RELATIVE GROWTH appreciated, is that the power function is only an approximate solution to the differential equations of time-dependent growth. Haldane pointed out to Huxley that a sum of parts growing at different rates with respect to body size cannot itself exactly follow an allometric relationship with body size (Reeve and Huxley, 1945). Lande (1985) elucidated the mathematical rationale underlying this statement and provided an expression predicting the amount of nonloglinear curvature in the allometry function resulting from this artifact. In spite of these difficulties, allometry might have a reasonably sound theoretical basis if tissue growth were always a uniformly multiplicative process, but unfortunately even this is rarely the case. The physiological mechanisms of differential growth are only partly understood, and for certain aspects, such as competition among different structures and specific organ competence in relation to age, the data appear to be partly conflicting (Reeve and Huxley, 1945). In addition, many complex developmental phenomena, such as the de nova deposition of membrane or dermal bone by osteoblasts within the dermis, behave very differently from simple multiplicative growth and yet still seem to conform to the model of simple allometry. Despite these exceptions and theoretical difficulties, Huxley's expression of simple allometry remains the most general and meaningful expression of relative growth. LEVELS OF ALLOMETRIC SCALING Although the primary emphasis of Problems of Relative Growth, as its name suggests, was centered on relative changes in body form during ontogeny, there is a surprising amount of empirical evidence that morphological variation among individuals at comparable stages of growth, and even among populations and species, conforms well to the allometric model (Cheverud, 1982; Lande, 1985; Jungers, 1985). Huxley intimated these various levels of variation, but the terminology and the distinctions among them developed later (Cock, 1966; Gould, 1966). Individual deviations in development, including slight differences in allometric growth gradients among individuals, produce phenotypic variation among individuals within a population. At any particular age or stage of development, Huxley's power function can be used to describe size-dependent variations in body form among individuals, which often conform reasonably well to patterns of loglinear variation. The

STUDY OF ALLOMETRY SINCE HUXLEY

Ii

resulting relationships, if they are described well by the allometric model, are called static allometries to distinguish them from ontogenetic allometries estimated from longitudinal data (measurements taken from the same individuals at several ages) or from variation among separate age groups. The static allometry of adults within a population depends on the variances and covariances of the traits, which can be expressed in terms of their initial values and growth increments (Lande, 1985 ). Such character associations are often fairly weak (Smith, 1981a) and have no necessary relationship to the average ontogenetic trajectory within a population, except in the biologically trivial case in which all variation among individuals corresponds exactly to different degrees of progression along a common developmental pathway. In fact, static allometric relationships often "cut across" average ontogenetic trajectories. Although the distinction between ontogenetic and static allometry is important conceptually, in practice the two are often confused and discussed as though they were synonymous. Their analyses have often been assumed to yield similar results (Simpson et al., 1960), but, despite the general positive association often observed between them (Cochard, 1981; Cheverud, 1982; Klingenberg and Zimmermann, 1992), there is no reason to expect them to be perfectly associated. Cock (1966) and Cheverud (1982) have given theoretical and empirical examples of major differences between ontogenetic and static allometry; in fact, their actual values might be quite different even when they are positively correlated (Leamy and Bradley, 1982). The term evolutionary allometry is used to describe allometric relationships among characters in closely related populations or species. Such covariances often result from secondary adaptations to evolutionary changes in body size (Rensch, 1959; Gould, 1966). Although, in its strict sense, evolutionary allometry refers to character covariances among individuals at corresponding ontogenetic stages in the population or species, the term is often used to describe variation among individuals at mixed stages, particularly when a large proportion of interspecific variation can be accounted for by allometric size variation (e.g., Strauss, 1984, 1985; Houck et al., 1990). Most studies of evolutionary allometry have investigated the static character covariances among species assumed to be

lii

PROBLEMS OF RELATIVE GROWTH

closely related (e.g., McKinney and Schoch, 1985; Strauss, 1985). The covariances among sets of species, however, might be very different from those within particular phylogenetic lineages, for at least two reasons: first, because covariances among extant species are insensitive to the directions and temporal sequences of phylogenetic character transformation (Donoghue, 1989; Martins and Garland, 1991); second, because species cannot be treated as independent observations if they are drawn from a common hierarchically structured phylogeny, as any traits held in common may be a result of common ancestry rather than, or in addition to, attainment due to independent adaptation (Felsenstein, 1985). For this reason one might also recognize a more restrictive kind of character covariance, phylogenetic allometry, describing variation within a single evolutionary lineage across time. Phylogenetic allometries can be inferred by parsimoniously "mapping" continuously varying character values onto phylogenetic trees and characterizing their covariances across time (Maddison, 1991; Martins and Garland, 1991; Lynch, 1991; Strauss 1992a, 1992b). Rieppel (1990) suggested that these three levels of allometry are closely interrelated. For example, any evolutionary change in morphology must be reducible at some level to a corresponding change in ontogeny, and the converse is also likely to be true. Evolutionary change also depends on heritable static variation of morphological traits in various ontogenetic stages, produced in turn by ontogenetic variation (Lande, 1979). APPLICATIONS OF ALLOMETRIC SCALING The concept of allometric scaling has had applications in three different biological arenas: morphology and morphogenesis, physiology, and ecology. Morphological Scaling There have been four interrelated applications of allometry in the study of morphology and morphogenesis (Atchley, 1983): (1) prediction of correlated evolutionary changes from the extrapolation of within-population covariances to interpopulational and interspecific covariances; (2) use as a "criterion of subtraction" (Gould, 1977) to correct for allometric effects in studies of shape differences among species; (3) use as

STUDY OF ALLOMETRY SINCE HUXLEY

liii

an indication of taxonomic relationships; and (4) use as a tool for evaluating functional relationships. 1. The prediction of correlated evolutionary changes has been a primary application of allometry. As formulated by Gould (197 5), the idea is that, if intraspecific allometry represents the correlated variability upon which evolution works, we should be able to determine the allometric rates of change of morphological traits following from changes in body size, if size alone is the object of selection. A caveat to such studies, based on the theory of quantitative genetics, is the demonstration by Lande (1979, 1985) that no necessary correspondence exists between patterns of static phenotypic variation within populations and evolutionary variation, even when selection acts only on body size. 2. Related to the use of allometry as a predictor of morphological change is its use as a parsimonious "null hypothesis" in accounting for observed patterns of variation among individuals. If it can be shown that the relative shapes or sizes of a structure in two or more species conform to an expected allometric dependence on body size, the dependence can then be subsumed in the more general explanation of size variation, and no special explanation (particularly one invoking adaptation) is needed to account for the differences in the structure per se (Gould and Lewontin, 1979; Sweet, 1980; Strauss, 1984). Perhaps the most striking examples are the analyses of antler size in the extinct "Irish elk" (Gould, 197 4), and the reconsideration of horn size in titanotheres (McKinney and Schoch, 1985). In contrast, notable departures from otherwise consistent allometric trends may be of particular value in identifying and understanding functional adaptations. White and Gould (1965) and Gould (1971) considered methods for quantifying departures from expected allometry. 3. If the ontogenetic allometric coefficient for some morphological trait is characteristic of a population or species, there is no reason in principle why the coefficient might not be used as a "character" for assessing taxonomic relationships. Strauss (1992b) characterized the larval and juvenile-to-adult growth patterns of several species of poeciliid fishes and examined their utility as phylogenetic characters. While parsimonious trees constructed from larval allometries were congruent with comparable trees based on conventional systematic criteria, trees constructed from juvenile-to-adult allometries were in-

liv

PROBLEMS OF RELATIVE GROWTH

congruent with these, reflecting ecophenotypic convergence in body form rather than phylogenetic relationships. Much more needs to be done to assess the potential for using "dynamic" characteristics, such as ontogenetic allometries, for phylogenetic inference. 4. The application of allometric models for evaluating functional relationships dates in principle at least back to Galileo (Galilei, 1954 edition), who elaborated upon the idea that the dimensions of terrestrial organisms should be dictated by the physical demands of holding up and moving the body against the force of gravity. The analysis of scaling relationships in locomotion has been a particularly active field for the past twenty years (Sleigh, 1974; Wu et al., 1975; Rainey, 1976; Alexander and Goldspink, 1977; Pedley, 1977; Norberg, 1981; Norberg and Rayner, 1987; Biewener, 1983, 1990). McMahon (197 3) first proposed that terrestrial organisms, including both plants and animals, exhibit elastic similarity rather than geometric similarity. Whereas the simple allometric model rather vaguely predicts that body proportions should change in some simple fashion with size in all organisms, McMahon's model concerns terrestrial vertebrates, particularly mammals, which are subject to gravitational forces that affect both posture and locomotion. The model is therefore much more specific, predicting, for example, that the length of weight-bearing elements should vary by the twothirds power of their diameter. Hence, if the long bones of mammalian limbs are of a constant density across species and comprise a constant fraction of body weight, their lengths and diameters should vary as the one-fourth and three-eighths power of body weight, respectively. McMahon (197 5 ), Alexander and his colleagues (Alexander, 1977; Maloiy et al., 1979; Alexander et al., 1979), and Biewener (1983) studied such allometric regularities in the limb proportions of mammals. They found that the model of elastic similarity holds very well for the long limb bones of bovids (cattle, antelopes, and their relatives), but that the predictions do not seem to hold nearly as well for the limb bones of other mammalian groups. In fact, Biewener concluded, from his study of thirty-two species of land mammals, that the diameters and lengths of the long bones scale almost as predicted by geometric similarity (or isometry) rather than elastic similarity. Similarly, Prothero (1992) applied an idealized geometric model of mammalian body form (consisting of cylinders

STUDY OF ALLOMETRY SINCE HUXLEY

lv

of specified length and diameter, representing three body regions: head and trunk, forelimbs, and hindlimbs) to morphometric data on a variety of mammals, and concluded that a model of simple isometry adequately fits the mammalian data over a substantial weight range. It must be kept in mind, however, that isometry in this context is a null hypothesis, which can be falsified but not necessarily corroborated by the data. Because the conclusion of geometric similarity is at odds with a large number of morphometric studies of terrestrial vertebrates demonstrating fine-scale allometric variation within and among species, it may be that the measurement and modeling of gross head, trunk, and limb lengths, across species ranging from mice to elephants, are simply insufficiently resolved.

Physiological Scaling An extensive literature has developed during the past halfcentury concerned with the demonstration and analysis of physiological and energetic scaling (Hemmingsen, 1960; Kleiber, 1961; Stahl, 1962, 1967). More recently, biologists have undertaken integrative studies that seek to provide formal, functional explanations for these scaling regularities, particularly in the context of the ecological roles and demands of organisms (Peters, 1983; Calder, 1984; Gittleman, 1985, 1986; Reiss, 1989). A now-classic example is McMahon's demonstration (1973) that the scaling of metabolic rate and the rates of heartbeat and breathing can be derived from criteria of elastic similarity that are characteristic of the locomotor system of terrestrial vertebrates. McMahon's work also emphasizes the importance of dynamic rather than static loading principles in the functional design of the muscular and skeletal systems of tetrapod vertebrates. Ecological Scaling As noted above, the allometric power function is the solution of the differential equation relating the specific growth rates of two traits, X and Y, with respect to time. By analogy, the same time-dependent equations may describe the growth of populations as well as of morphological traits. Turner (197 8; Griffiths and Sandland, 1982) generalized the allometry of relative population growth by modeling it by means of the conservative and non-conservative Lotka-Volterra kinetic equations; he found the fit to the non-conservative model in general to be

lvi

PROBLEMS OF RELATIVE GROWTH

relatively better (in relation to the number of parameters in the model) than that for the simple allometric model. Antonelli (1985) showed that Huxley's model holds for logistic growth as well. Such models can be used, for example, to study the allometry of population cycling (Peterson et al., 1984) or the ecological scaling of herbivory with primary productivity (Cyr and Pace, 1993). STATISTICAL ISSUES AND THE FITTING OF ALLOMETRIC MODELS There has been a great deal of concern about the uses and abuses of various statistical techniques, because statistical regression procedures are so widely applied in allometric studies and the resulting allometric coefficients are often used for comparing relative growth patterns among different traits or taxonomic groups. Calculating the position of a line of best fit through a scatter of data is subject to at least three critical problems: (1) the use of linear versus non-linear regression methods (and the associated problem of logarithms); (2) assumptions about the "error structure" of the data, which in turn determine the particular form of regression to be used; and (3) the non-independence of points representing individuals or species. The spectrum of choices that need to be made for an empirical study of allometry confronts the biologist with "a confusing plethora of models and options" (German and Meyers, 1989). Linear versus Non-linear Regression There is a variety of ways to estimate allometric coefficients (Simpson et al., 1960; Kidwell and Chase, 1967; Manaster and Manaster, 1975; Siem and Saether, 1983), but with very few exceptions studies of bivariate allometry use standard leastsquares predictive regression. After conversion of the two variables X and Y to logarithms to linearize Huxley's model, the slope coefficient of Y on X is a direct estimate of k. Although typically unimportant in allometric studies, the Y intercept (or an equivalent adjusted mean, adjusted to a value of X other than zero) is often of value in studies of relative shape differences between groups (Bookstein et al., 1985). There is a significant statistical problem, however, with the fitting of the log-transformed, linearized version of the allometric model, a problem that was pointed out by Zar (1968). The

STUDY OF ALLOMETRY SINCE HUXLEY

lvii

linear regression finds the "best-fitting" line that minimizes the sum of squared deviations (residuals) of the data from it, but the residuals that are minimized are the transformed residuals, not those of the original data. This introduces a systematic bias into the calculations, with the result that the regression may provide only crude estimates of the "true" parameters (including the allometric coefficient), and the approximation is frequently poor. There are two ways in which this bias can be overcome: by using a correction factor to counteract it (Sprugel, 1983) or by using an iterative nonlinear regression procedure to fit a curvilinear function to the original rather than the transformed data. There are three classes of techniques in current use for fitting data to nonlinear functions (Glass, 1969): the Taylor-series expansion method, wherein successive corrections to parameter estimates are made until the solution converges; the gradient methods; and Marquardt's procedure, which combines the best features of the first two techniques. All of these have been used at one time or another in the fitting of allometric models. The decision whether to use a linear or nonlinear procedure is not a trivial one, and there are criteria other than statistical fit to consider. For example, loglinear models are particularly amenable to descriptive analysis in cases in which the trajectory proves to be irregular or polyphasic. Inflection points, representing potential shifts in allometric growth patterns during development, are much easier to detect with linearized plots than with nonlinear ones. It is often beneficial to use loglinear plots in an exploratory way to examine the structure of the data, followed by "confirmatory" statistical fitting to derive the best possible statistical estimates of the parameters of the model. Regardless of the method used to fit the allometric model to empirical data, it is worth emphasizing that three kinds of information about the regression procedure are needed: estimates of the parameters; estimates of the uncertainty of the estimates; and a statistical measure of goodness-of-fit. When the third item suggests that the model is an unlikely match to the data, the first two items are probably worthless. Unfortunately, many researchers never proceed beyond the parameter estimates; they deem a fit acceptable if a graph of the data and model "looks good," an approach known sardonically among statisticians as "chi-by-eye."

lviii

PROBLEMS OF RELATIVE GROWTH

Error Structure Whether linear or non-linear, the standard least-squares predictive regression models (so-called Type I procedures) assume an "independent" X variable, measured without error, and a "dependent" variable Y, subject to sampling "error," which includes both measurement error and individual biological variability. However, because in morphometric studies X is seldom without measurement error or biological variation, the regression estimate of the allometric coefficient k will be biased downward. The magnitude of this bias is a function of the amount of error in X (Cock, 1966). Due to this bias, many authors have proposed the use of regression methods that allow for variation in both variables (Type II procedures), such as major-axis regression, reduced (standardized) major-axis regression, or linear functional regression (Teissier, 1948; Kermack and Haldane, 1950; Ricker, 197 3, 1984; Kuhry and Marcus, 1977; Rayner, 1985; Mattfeldt and Mall, 1987; Hofman, 1988; Pagel and Harvey, 1988, 1989; Jolicoeur, 1990; Jolicoeur and Ducharme, 1992; Kimura, 1992). All of these simultaneously minimize the vertical and horizontal deviations from the allometry line, but differ in their assumptions about the relative errors associated with the two variables. Each method assumes a particular value for the ratio of error variances, and an incorrect assumption will cause systematic bias in the corresponding estimate of the slope. The various forms of regression are nearly equivalent when the characters are very highly correlated, as with most ontogenetic data, but can be dissimilar in slope when the characters are only weakly correlated, as for the static allometry of adults within a population. The choice is not indifferent from a biological standpoint: in a study by Wolpoff (1982), for example, negative allometry (k < 1) for a particular dental trait was inferred based on a Type I regression, whereas positive allometry (k > 1) for the same trait was indicated by a Type II regression. Riska (1991) has argued that the regression problem has never been adequately resolved because the choice is not completely a statistical one; it requires explicit biological models analyzed within a phylogenetic framework. Phylogenetic Non-independence of Observations The phylogenetic framework leads to the third problem, which is that evolutionary changes occurring in the past are

STUDY OF ALLOMETRY SINCE HUXLEY

lix

likely to be inherited by present-day descendants. Thus the species within one genus, for example, are more likely to resemble one another than they are to resemble species in other genera, due simply to their common ancestry. Statistically, they do not represent independent data points, which violates an important assumption underlying regression models (as well as most other statistical procedures). In the same way, genera within taxonomic families are likely to be more similar to each other than are genera from different families, and so on. Of course, taxonomic groupings such as genera and families (and even species) can be coarse and arbitrary; but given a phylogenetic tree specifying more refined relationships among species, taxonomic dependence is likely among all branches emanating from a given node. Felsenstein (1985) provided one possible solution to the problem of taxonomic dependence. Given a phylogenetic tree, differences between sister taxa derived from a common node will have evolved independently of differences elsewhere on the tree. Given a bifurcating tree (with two sister taxa emanating from every node), there will be n - 1 independent comparisons, where n is the number of extant species. If the tree is not fully resolved, other solutions are possible (Grafen, 1989; Harvey and Pagel, 1990; Martins and Garland, 1991). NON-LOGLINEAR ALLOMETRY Despite the wide application of Huxley's model of simple loglinear allometry, a survey of the literature of allometry reveals many examples of bivariate scaling that do not conform to simple allometry. Such exceptions are generally of two kinds: cases of polyphasic loglinear allometry, in which ontogenetic trajectories can be divided into loglinear portions representing different portions of the life cycle; and complex allometry, in which ontogenetic trajectories are curvilinear on a log-log plot. Polyphasic Loglinear Allometry The existence of allometric trajectories having two or more loglinear phases was first noted by Huxley (1932) in the relationships between the lengths of the chelae and carapace in fiddler crabs, as well as in the mouthpart dimensions of ants, beetles, and other insects. Many other examples have been described since then in the size-scaling of morphological and physiological data (e.g., Teissier, 1937, 1960; Gould, 1966;

lx

PROBLEMS OF RELATIVE GROWTH

Phillipson, 1981; Hamburger et al., 1983; Peters, 1983; Schmidt-Nielsen, 1984; Strauss and Fuiman, 1985; Creighton and Strauss, 1986; Guerrant, 1988; Forbes, 1989; Lauder and Reilly, 1990; Reilly and Lauder, 1990; Brooks, 1991; Strauss, 1992b). An early, now-classic example is that of Wilson (1953), who developed a scenario hypothesizing evolutionary pathways in the development of caste polymorphism in ants, wherein diphasic and triphasic patterns represent transitions from monomorphism to caste dimorphism. Many other kinds of non-loglinear developmental trajectories have been described, including those accounting for ontogenetic attainment of sexual dimorphism and for the reorganization of growth patterns in the transition of larval stages to juvenile and adult forms. The inflection points separating the loglinear phases are often associated with such critical developmental periods during ontogeny (Kluge and Strauss, 1985). Various techniques have been proposed for determining the inflection points of polyphasic functions (Somerton, 1980a, 1980b; Forbes and Lopez, 1989). To determine the extent to which various morphological structures might follow such polyphasic trajectories and lead to expressions of sexual dimorphism in vertebrates, Brooks (1991) formulated a hierarchical set of ontogenetic-trajectory models under the assumption that juveniles, mature females, and mature males each possessed growth patterns that could be characterized by simple allometry, but that attainment of sexual maturity is correlated with "instantaneous" shifts in the pattern of allometric growth. The "null" model is that of simple allometry, in which female and male allometric patterns for some morphological trait are merely extensions of the juvenile pattern; males and females would exhibit sexual dimorphism in this case only if one sex grew to a larger size than the other. A second model describes the case in which one sex continues along the juvenile trajectory, while the other undergoes a shift in allometric growth patterns such that it diverges from the juvenile trajectory. This would typically result in sexual dimorphism even if both sexes grew to the same body size; sexual monomorphism could result only if males and females grew to "compensatory" sizes, exhibiting the same relative size of the trait, that is, the same "shape," at different body sizes. Three other models are also possible by allowing both males and females to diverge from the juvenile trajectory. The simplest of these results is when males and females diverge in

STUDY OF ALLOMETRY SINCE HUXLEY

lxi

the same way at the same body size. This is a four-parameter description that, like the null model, produces sexual monomorphism unless one sex grows to a larger size. If males and females diverge in different ways at either the same size or different sizes, three loglinear segments are needed to characterize the variation. The purpose of formulating these alternate descriptive models is to be able to best characterize the ontogenetic allometric pattern for a morphological or physiological trait. This is a statistical problem that can be solved by fitting the various models to a set of data, allowing the point of inflection to be estimated by the data, and deciding which of them best describes the observations (accounting for the number of parameters) in terms of producing the smallest residual variance. Brooks applied these models to test the widely held belief that, in continuously growing vertebrates, sexual dimorphism arises from allometric changes in males at the onset of maturity while females continue along the path set by juvenile growth. In fitting the models on a character-by-character basis to cross-sectional morphometric data for four fish species, however, she found that different character complexes grow in different ways; that divergence from juvenile allometry occurs as frequently in female traits as in male traits; and that elaboration of a trait in one sex does not guarantee that this sex is the one that diverges from the juvenile allometric pattern.

Complex Allometry It may often be the case (possibly more often than realized) that the allometric coefficient is itself a continuous function of body size, so that an allometric trajectory appears as a curvilinear function on a log-log plot. Laird (1965) and Wingert (1969) observed such patterns of "complex allometry" and attempted to model them using Gompertz and logistic timedependent growth curves; in both cases they observed satisfactory fit. Turner (1978) and Griffiths and Sandland (1982, 1984) attempted to describe complex allometry based on the Lotka-Volterra equations, and also observed adequate empirical fit. None of these models, however, provides a practical test of the null hypothesis of simple allometry. Jolicoeur and his co-workers (Lebeau et al., 1986; Jolicoeur and Pirlot, 1988; Jolicoeur, 1989) argued that the Gompertz and logistic models are inadequate for describing morphological growth because both lack flexibility at the upper bound,

lxii

PROBLEMS OF RELATIVE GROWTH

neither is constrained to pass through the origin, and the inflection points of these sigmoid-shaped curves are fixed at arbitrary levels. They instead proposed a modified version of the Putter-Bertalanffy model, which was originally derived from physiological assumptions about metabolic growth, and from this time-dependent model derived an expression for complex allometry. Jolicoeur (1989) also derived from the Gompertz model a companion expression, which he observed to have better empirical fit to several data sets, and documented a number of cases in which the model of complex allometry provided significantly better fit than that of simple allometry (Dawood et al., 1988; Jolicoeur, 1989; Cabana et al., 1990). The only other application of Jolicoeur's model was by Strauss and Altig (1991), who failed to find significant evidence of complex allometry in the growth of tadpoles. MULTIVARIATE ALLOMETRY Perhaps the single most significant advance in the analysis of allometric variation has been the generalization of Huxley's bivariate model to one encompassing three or more variables simultaneously. Huxley's model involves the two idealized metric traits, X and Y. Although X and Yin principle can be any measurable features of an organism, the "independent variable" X is typically chosen to be some measurement that reflects (i.e., is assumed to be a proxy for) the overall size of the body (such as total body length) or of the section of the body being studied (such as cranial length, or total head plus thorax length). In examining the allometry of organ weights, Huxley (1924, 1932) used X to represent total body weight minus the organ weight Y. A significant problem with such bivariate analyses is that the allometric coefficient is a function of the particular sizevariable selected. For example, in studies of human dental allometry the allometric coefficients derived when body mass is used as a measure of body size differ from those obtained when cranial length or skull length is the reference dimension (Smith, 1981b). This is true even when the various size measures are highly correlated with one another. For this reason the selection of an appropriate size variable has been said to be "the fundamental and frequently neglected problem confronting all studies of allometry" (Jungers and German, 1981). A great deal of energy has been expended in discussions of the appropriate size measures to apply in particular studies or

STUDY OF ALLOMETRY SINCE HUXLEY

lxiii

with particular groups of organisms. However, the approach that has generally proven most fruitful has been to use a composite measure of body size, reflecting the average value of a suite of traits measured for each individual. Following Sewell Wright (1932), such a composite measure, when it is discovered from (or fitted to) the data rather than defined a priori, is called general size (although Bookstein [1989] refined the terminology considerably). The use of a general-size measure, which estimates the "robustness" of an organism, is preferable to the use of particular measured traits for at least two reasons (Strauss, 1987): because individual characters are generally statistically "noisier" than composite traits, exhibiting more individual or random variation that is averaged out in the composite; and because any particular character is likely to be allometric with respect to general body size. Composite traits may be of two kinds: simple (unweighted) sums or averages, or weighted sums or averages. A simple average is merely a special case of a weighted average in which each measured character is weighted equally; if the character is weighted, it is typically weighted by its own value, thus giving more weight to longer than to shorter measurements. A simple average might be an arithmetic mean of the raw mensural data or an arithmetic mean of the logtransformed data. In the latter case it is equivalent to the geometric mean of the raw data, which again gives more weight to longer measurements than to shorter ones. When the mensural data for an individual consist of distances among sets of morphological "landmarks" (specific anatomical points that are assumed to be homologous, or at least comparable, among individuals; Bookstein et al., 1985), a measure of general size that has some desirable statistical properties is the centroid size of Bookstein (1991), the sum of the squared pairwise distances among landmarks. A significant benefit of all such composite measures is that they can be calculated independently for each individual without considering the rest of the sample (Strauss and Bookstein, 1985). Paradoxically, this is also their primary disadvantage in allometric studies. Because there is no information about allometric variation in the form of a single individual at a single time, the forms of all individuals within a sample or population must be used to characterize relative changes in characters during growth. The desire for a composite measure of body size taking ontogenetic allometry into consideration has been

lxiv

PROBLEMS OF RELATIVE GROWTH

satisfied by the application of multivariate statistics (Bookstein, 1989). The primary tools for this purpose are factor analysis, originated by Wright in the 1920s (Wright, 1932, 1954; Teissier, 1955, 1960; Baker, 1954; Hopkins, 1966), and principal component analysis, applied to the characterization of allometric size variation by Jolicoeur (1963a, Jolicoeur and Mosimann, 1960). The input for each analysis is the same: a matrix containing information about the covariances among all measured variables, values that characterize the associations among variables in terms of the original scale of measurement. (Note that correlations are computed by removing information about scale of measurement, which also removes information about relative rates of change; thus, correlations are not generally useful in allometric studies.) The covariances are calculated using the log-transformed data to linearize any allometric relationships among the variables. The two techniques differ primarily in that factor analysis (in the sense used here) is a "confirmatory" procedure, based on the fitting of an explicit model to the data and invoking various assumptions about the nature of the error components, whereas principal-component analysis is "exploratory," simply identifying the major patterns of variation within the data. Thus the principal-component approach will correctly identify a size component only when size variation is the major source of morphological variation in the data. Various modifications of principal-component analysis have been suggested over the years to ensure its performance for multiple groups and for specific kinds of data (e.g., Burnaby, 1966; Humphries et al., 1981; Somers, 1986, 1989; Rohlf and Bookstein, 1987; Airoldi and Flury, 1988a, 1988b; Thorpe. 1988). Regardless of the computational details, there are four important conceptual points about these methods. First, in the multivariate approach, size is viewed as an explanation of the measured data rather than a characteristic of them; as size increases, it "causes" the measurable traits of an organism to become larger together, each at its own allometric rate. In the language of factor analysis, general size is a latent variable that can only be estimated, not observed. Second, the result of either factor analysis or principal-component analysis is a function that is a linear combination of the original measured variables: in other words, a weighted sum that can be calculated for each individual from the coefficients of the function, which

STUDY OF ALLOMETRY SINCE HUXLEY

lxv

are simply the weights for each variable. If the weighted sum were divided by the number of variables, it would be a weighted average. The important point is that the formula providing the weights is not known in advance of the data analysis, but rather is derived from the data. Because the linear combination specifies the position of a straight line or vector through the geometric space defined by the variables, the factor or component is often said to be a size vector. The vector is assumed to account completely for allometric variation, which can be described by the direction of the line. The weighted sum (or average) is a score (or projection) of an individual on this axis, which provides a measure of its general body size. Third, the factor or principal component provides information not only about multivariate size, but also about multivariate allometry. The unscaled coefficients (also called loadings or direction cosines) are regression coefficients, and thus are estimates of the rates of change of individual characters with a change in general size (Leamy and Bradley, 1982; Lande, 1985). For this reason they are often called multivariate allometric coefficients. Fourth, just as there has been much argument about which particular univariate measure might best represent body size in an allometric analysis, there has also been much discussion about the appropriate multivariate measure. In terms of allometric models, the primary point of contention is whether, in studies of shape variation among different groups (such as populations, species, or ecological assemblages), it is desirable for a measure of general size to reflect allometric shape differences (Sprent, 1972; Mosimann, 1970; Mosimann and James, 1979; Humphries et al., 1981; Bookstein et al., 1985; Somers, 1986, 1989; Sundberg, 1989). An interesting aspect of the application of multivariate allometric coefficients is that the information about relative bivariate growth is not lost among the multivariate coefficients. For any two characters, the ratio of their coefficients is proportional to the coefficient k in Huxley's function (Jolicoeur, 1963b). The pairwise coefficients are simply rescaled by an appropriate constant of proportionality, chosen so that the overall rate of change is defined as isometry (Hills, 1982; Shea, 1985). There are a number of uses for multivariate allometric vectors, once they have been estimated, and these can be classified into single-group and multiple-group applications. For a single group, it may be of interest to test statistically whether

lxvi

PROBLEMS OF RELATIVE GROWTH

the observed vector differs significantly from an idealized vector of isometry. This is the multivariate extension of the test for whether a single bivariate allometric coefficient k differs from unity, the value corresponding to isometry. Two related tests have been used for this purpose. Asymptotic procedures (i.e., assuming infintely large sample sizes) for testing hypotheses about the directions of vectors for populations were developed in the mid-1960s (Anderson, 1963) and were the basis of Jolicoeur's (1963b, 1984) tests for allometric data. These are powerful statistical tests but are based on the rather rigorous assumption that the population from which the data have been sampled is distributed according to a multivariatenormal distribution. An alternate procedure is to examine directly the angular deviation between the observed allometry vector and a vector of isometry (Kohn and Atchley, 1988). Bryant and Meffert (1988) developed a fairly robust "bootstrapping" (randomization) procedure to determine indirectly whether the angular deviation is significant. Procedures similar to those used for single-group tests can be used to determine whether, and to what degree, the multivariate size vectors for two groups differ in orientation (Reyment et al., 1984). This is a very important test, because the assumption of parallel size vectors underlies the various procedures for discovering "size-invariant" or "size-free" shape differences among groups (Burnaby, 1966; Humphries et al., 1981; Rohlf and Bookstein, 198 7); if two or more groups differ significantly in their multivariate allometries (i.e., if their multivariate growth patterns are sufficiently different), the shape differences between any two individuals will depend on how large they are. An important recent application of multivariate allometric size vectors is that used by Zelditch (1987, 1988, 1989) in testing for the existence of "secondary factors" expressing morphological integration during rodent development. Zelditch used a confirmatory factor-analysis procedure to test whether various alternate developmental models, based on embryonic tissue origins or geometric or functional covariances, fit several sets of morphometric growth data any better than the null model of allometric growth. Although she failed to find evidence of such integration, possibly due to low statistical power resulting from small sample sizes, her work has established a context for examining patterns of secondary covariation that should be pursued with other organisms.

STUDY OF ALLOMETRY SINCE HUXLEY

lxvii

ENVOI Huxley's Problems of Relative Growth is a classic work in the best sense of the term: scholarly, insightful, and elegantly written. It can be favorably compared to the influence of Darwin's Origin of Species in that many, perhaps most, of the current problems in organismic morphology were suggested or predicated in some way by Huxley. A great deal of innovative and critical work in relative growth and form has been accomplished in the past sixty years, some of it by Huxley himself after the publication of Problems of Relative Growth. This later work has clarified and extended Huxley's basic models of allometry and growthgradients to much wider arenas, has anchored his descriptive work more firmly in the cellular processes of growth and development, and has extrapolated the allometric model to the realm of multivariate statistics-made practical only with the advent of modern digital computers. Due to the current widespread interest in developmental patterns, however, particularly with regard to heterochronic changes in such patterns over evolutionary time, Huxley's work remains a valuable reference and is cited perhaps as much today as it ever was. Problems of Relative Growth demonstrates the persistent problems confronting morphologists as they struggle to explain the tremendous diversity of organic forms. REFERENCES Adolph, E. F. 1949. Quantitative relations in the physiological constitutions of animals. Science 109:579-585. Airoldi, J.-P., and B. K. Flury. 1988a. An application of common principal component analysis to cranial morphometry of Microtus californicus and M. ochrogaster (Mammalia, Rodentia). ]. Zool. (London) 216:21-36. - - . 1988b. An application of common principal component analysis to cranial morphometry of Microtus californicus and M. ochrogaster (Mammalia, Rodentia): further remarks. ]. Zool. (London) 216: 21-36. Alexander, R. McN., and G. Goldspink, eds. 1977. Mechanics and Energetics of Animal Locomotion. London: Chapman and Hall. Alexander, R. McN., A. S. Jayes, G. M. 0. Maloiy, and E. M. Wathuta. 1979. Allometry of the limb bones of mammals from shrews (Sorex) to elephant (Loxodonta). ]. Zool. (London) 189:305-314. Anderson, T. W. 1963. Asymptotic theory for principal component analysis. Ann. Math. Stat. 34:122-148. Antonelli, P. L. 1985. Non-Euclidean allometry and the growth of forests

lxviii

PROBLEMS OF RELATIVE GROWTH and corals. In Mathematical Essays on Growth and the Emergence of Form, ed. P. L. Antonelli, 45-47. Edmonton: University of Al-

berta Press. Atchley, W. R. 1983. Some genetic aspects of morphometric variation. In Numerical Taxonomy, ed. J. Felsenstein, 346-363. Berlin: Springer-Verlag. Atchley, W. R., and B. K. Hall. 1991. A model for development and evolution of complex morphological structures. Biol. Rev. 66:101-157. Baker, G. A. 1954. Factor analysis of relative growth. Growth 18: 137-143. Biewener, A. A. 1983. Allometry of quadrupedal locomotion: The scaling of duty factor, bone curvature and limb orientation to body size. ]. Exp. Biol. 105:147-171. - - . 1990. Biomechanics of mammalian terrestrial locomotion. Science 250:1097-1103. Bookstein, F. L. 1989. "Size and shape": A comment on semantics. Syst. Zool. 38:173-180. - - . 1991. Morphometric Tools for Landmark Data: Geometry and Biology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Bookstein, F. L., B. Chernoff, R. L. Elder, J. M. Humphries, Jr., G. R. Smith, and R. E. Strauss. 1985. Morphometrics in Evolutionary Biology. Acad. Nat. Sci. Phila., Spec. Pub!. 15. Brody, S. 1945. Bioenergetics and Growth. New York: Reinhold. Brooks, M. J. 1991. The ontogeny of sexual dimorphism: Quantitative models and a case study in labrisomid blennies (Teleostei: Paraclinus.) Syst. Zool. 40:271-283. Bryant, E. H., and L. M. Meffert. 1988. Effect of an experimental bottleneck on morphological integration in the housefly. Evolution 42:698-707. Burnaby, T. P. 1966. Growth-invariant discriminant functions and generalized distances. Biometrics 22:96-110. Cabana, T., P. Jolicoeur, and G. Baron. 1990. Brain and body growth and allometry in the Mongolian gerbil (Meriones unguiculatus). Growth Devel. Ag. 54:23-30. Calder, W. A. 1984. Size, Function, and Life History. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Champy, C. 1924. Sexualite et Hormones. Paris: G. Doin. Cheverud, J. M. 1982. Relationships among ontogenetic, static and evolutionary allometry. Am.]. Phys. Anthropol. 59:139-149. Clark, L. B., and A. H. Hersh. 1939. A study of relative growth in Notonecta undulata. Growth 3:347-372. Cochard, L. 1981. Ontogenetic and intersexual dental allometry in the rhesus macaque. Am.]. Phys. Anthropol. 54:210. Cock, A. G. 1966. Genetical aspects of metrical growth and form in animals. Quart. Rev. Biol. 41:131-190. Creighton, G. K., and R. E. Strauss. 1986. Comparative patterns of growth and development in cricetine rodents and the evolution of ontogeny. Evolution 40:94-106. Crozier, W. J. 1914. The growth of the shell of the lamellibranch Dosinia

STUDY OF ALLOMETRY SINCE HUXLEY

lxix

discus (Reeve). Zoologische Jahrbucher Abteilung fur Anatomie 38:577-584. Crozier, W. J.. and S. Hecht. 1914. Correlations of weight, length, and other body measurements in the weakfish, Cynoscion regalis. Bull. U.S. Bur. Fish. 33:141-147. Cyr, H., and M. L. Pace. 1993. Magnitude and patterns of herbivory in aquatic and terrestrial ecosystems. Nature 361:148-150. Dawood, N., P. Jolicoeur, and S. D. Sharief. 1988. Postnatal brain growth and allometry in the rabbit Oryctolagus cuniculus. Growth Develop. Ag. 52:169-175. Donoghue, M. J. 1989. Phylogenies and the analysis of evolutionary sequences, with examples from seed plants. Evolution 43: 1137-1156. Druzinsky, R. E., and B. L. Manion. 1982. Allometry of the limbs of South American rodents and McMahon's theory of elastic similarity. Am. ]. Phys. Anthropol. 57:182. Dubois, E. 1914. Die Gegetzmassige Beziehung von Gehirn- zur Korpergrosse bei den Wirbeltieren. Zeitschr. Morphol. Anthropol. 25: 1-36. - - . 1918. On the relation between the quantities of the brain, the neurone and its parts, and the size of the body. Proc. Acad. Sci. (Amsterdam) 20:1328-1362. Felsenstein, J. 1985. Phylogenies and the comparative method. Amer. Natur. 125:1-15. Forbes, T. L. 1989. The importance of size-dependent processes in the ecology of deposit-feeding benthos. In The Ecology of Marine Deposit Feeders, ed. G. R. Lopez, J. Leuinton, and G. L. Taghon, 171-200. New York: Springer-Verlag. Forbes, T. L., and G. R. Lopez. 1989. Determination of critical periods in ontogenetic trajectories. Funct. Ecol. 3:625-632. Galilei, G. 1954. Dialogues Concerning Two New Sciences, trans. H. Crew and A. de Salvio. New York: Dover. Gerhart, J. C., et al. 1982. The cellular basis of morphogenetic change (Group Report). In Evolution and Development, ed. J. T. Bonner, 87-113. Berlin: Springer-Verlag. German, R. Z., and L. L. Meyers. 1989. The role of time and size in ontogenetic allometry. I. Review. Growth Develop. Ag. 53:101-106. Gittleman, J. L. 1985. Carnivore body size: ecological and taxonomic correlates. Oecologia 67:540-554. - - . 1986. Carnivore life history patterns: Allometric, phylogenetic, and ecological associations. Amer. Nat. 12 7: 744- 771. Glass, N. R. 1969. Discussion of calculation of power function with special reference to respiratory metabolism in fish. ]. Fish. Res. Bd. Canada 26:2643-2650. Gould, S. J. 1966. Allometry and size in ontogeny and phylogeny. Biol. Rev. 41:587-640. - - . 1971. Geometric similarity in allometric growth: a contribution to the problem of scaling in the evolution of size. Am. Nat. 105: 113-136.

lxx

PROBLEMS OF RELATIVE GROWTH

- - . 1974. The origin and function of "bizarre" structures: Antler size and skull size in the "Irish Elk," Megaloceros giganteus. Evolution 28: 191-220. - - . 1975. Allometry in primates, with emphasis on scaling and the evolution of the brain. Contrib. Primat. 5:244-293. - - . 1977. Ontogeny and Phylogeny. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Gould, S. J., and R. C. Lewontin. 1979. The spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian paradigm: A critique of the adaptionist programme. Proc. Roy. Soc. Land. (B) 205:581-598. Grafen, A. 1989. The phylogenetic regression. Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. Land. (B) 326:119-157. Griffiths, D., and R. Sandland. 1982. Allometry and multivariate growth revisited. Growth 46:1-11. - - . 1984. Fitting generalized allometric models to multivariate growth data. Biometrics 40:139-150. Guerrant, E. 0. 1988. Heterochrony in plants: the intersection of evolution ecology and ontogeny. In Heterochrony in Evolution, ed. M. L. McKinney, 111-133. New York: Plenum Press. Hamburger, K., F. M0hlenberg, A. Randl0v, and H. U. Riisgard. 1983. Size, 0 2 consumption, and growth in the mussel Mytilus edulis. Mar. Biol. 75:303-306. Harvey, P.H., and M. D. Pagel. 1990. The Comparative Method in Evolutionary Biology. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Hecht, S. 1913. The relation of weight to length in the smooth dogfish, Mustelus canis. Anat. Rec. 7:39-42. - - . 1916. Form and growth in fishes.]. Morphol. 27:379-400. Hemmingsen, A. M. 1960. Energy metabolism as related to body size and respiratory surfaces and its evolution. Repts. Steno Mem. Hosp. Nord. Insulin Lab. (Copenhagen) 9:7-110. Hills, M. 1982. Bivariate versus multivariate allometry: a note on a paper by Jungers and German. Am.]. Phys. Anthropol. 59:321-322. Hofman, M. A. 1988. Allometric scaling in palaeontology: A critical survey. Human Evol. 3:177-188. Hopkins, J. W. 1966. Some considerations in multivariate allometry. Biometrics 22:747-760. Houck, M.A., J. Gauthier, and R. E. Strauss. 1990. Allometric scaling in the earliest fossil bird, Archaeopteryx lithographica. Science 274: 195-198. Humphries, J. H., F. L. Bookstein, B. Chernoff, G. Smith, R. Elder, and S. Poss. 1981. Multivariate discrimination by shape in relation to size. Syst. Zool. 30:291-308. Huxley, J. S. 1924. Constant differential growth ratios and their significance. Nature 114:895-896. Jolicoeur, P. 1963a. The multivariate generalization of the allometry equation. Biometrics 19:497-499. - - . l 963b. The degree of generality of robustness in Martes americana. Growth 27:1-27. - - . 1984 Principal components, factor analysis, and multivariate al-

STUDY OF ALLOMETRY SINCE HUXLEY

lxxi

lometry: A small-sample direction test. Biometrics 40:685-690. - - . 1989. A simplified model for bivariate complex allometry.]. Theoret. Biol. 140:41-49. - - . 1990. Bivariate allometry: Interval estimation of the slopes of the ordinary and standardized normal major axes and structural relationship.]. Theoret. Biol. 144:275-285. Jolicoeur, P., and G. Ducharme. 1992. Bivariate allometry: Point estimation of the slope of the normal major axis. ]. Theoret. Biol. 154:35-41. Jolicoeur, P., and J. E. Mosimann. 1960. Size and shape variation in the painted turtle: A principal component analysis. Growth 24: 339-354. Jolicoeur, P., and P. Pirlot. 1988. Asymptotic growth and complex allometry of the brain and body in the white rat. Growth Develop. Ag. 52:3-9. Jungers, W. L. 1985. Size and Scaling in Primate Biology. New York: Plenum Press. Jungers, W. L., and R. Z. German. 1981. Ontogenetic and interspecific skeletal allometry in nonhuman primates: Bivariate versus multivariate analysis. Am.]. Phys. Antropol. 55:195-202. Katz, J. M. 1980. Allometry formula: A cellular model. Growth 44:89-96. Kermack, K. A., and J. B. S. Haldane. 1950. Organic correlation and allometry. Biometrika 37:30-41. Kidwell, J. F., and H.B. Chase. 1967. Fitting the allometric equation-a comparison of ten methods by computer simulation. Growth 31: 165-179. Kimura, D. K. 1992. Symmetry and scale dependence in functional relationship regression. Syst. Biol. 41:233-241. Klatt, B. 1919. Zur methodik vergleichender metrischer Untersuchungen, besonders des Herzgewichtes. Biol. Zentralbl. 39:406-438. Kleiber, M. 1961. The Fire of Life: An Introduction to Animal Energetics. New York: John Wiley and Sons. Klingenberg, C. P., and M. Zimmermann. 1992. Static, ontogenetic, and evolutionary allometry: A multivariate comparison in nine species of waterstriders. Amer. Nat. 140:601-620. Kluge, A. G. and R. E. Strauss. 1985. Ontogeny and systematics. Ann. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 16:24 7-268. Kohn, L.A. P., and W.R. Atchley. 1988. How similar are genetic correlation structures? Data from mice and rats. Evolution 42:467-481. Kuhry, B., and L. F. Marcus. 1977. Bivariate linear models in biometry. Syst. Zool. 26:201-209. Laird, A. K. 1965. Dynamics of relative growth. Growth 29:249-263. Laird, A. K., A. D. Barton, and S. A. Tyler. 1968. Growth and time: An interpretation of allometry. Growth 32:34 7-354. Lande, R. 1979. Quantitative genetic analysis of multivariate evolution, applied to brain: Body size allometry. Evolution 33:402-416. - - . 1985. Genetic and evolutionary aspects of allometry. In Size and Scaling in Primate Biology, ed. W. L. Jungers, 21-32. New York: Plenum Press.

lxxii

PROBLEMS OF RELATIVE GROWTH

Lauder, G. V., and S. M. Reilly. 1990. Metamorphosis of the feeding mechanism in tiger salamanders. (Ambystoma tigrinum): The ontogeny of cranial muscle mass. ]. Zool. (London) 222:59- 74. Leamy, L., and D. Bradley. 1982. Static and growth allometry of morphometric traits in randombred house mice. Evolution 36:1200-1212. Lebeau, B., P. Jolicoeur, G. Pageau, and E. J. Crossman. 1986. Asymptotic growth, egg production and trivariate allometry in Esox masquinongy Mitchill. Growth 50:185-200. Le Gros Clark, W. E., and P. B. Medawar, eds. 1945. Essays on Growth and Form. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Lumer, H. 1937. The consequences of sigmoid growth for relative growth function. Growth 1:140-154. Lynch, M. 1991. Methods for the analysis of comparative data in evolutionary biology. Evolution 45:1065-1080. Maddison, W. P. 1991. Squared-change parsimony reconstructions of ancestral states for continuous-valued characters on a phylogenetic tree. Syst. Zool. 40:304-314. Manaster, B. J .. and S. Manaster. 1975. Techniques for estimating allometric equations.]. Morph. 147:299-308. Martins, E. P., and T. Garland, Jr. 1991. Phylogenetic analyses of the correlated evolution of continuous characters: A simulation study. Evolution 45:534-557. Mattfeldt, T., and G. Mall. 1987. Statistical methods for growth allometric studies. Growth 51:86-102. McKinney, M. L., and R. M. Schoch. 1985. Titanothere allometry, heterochrony, and biomechanics: Revising an evolutionary classic. Evolution 39:1352-1363. McMahon, T. 1973. Size and shape in biology. Science 179:1201-1204. McMahon, T. A. 1975. Allometry and biomechanics: Limb bones in adult ungulates. Am. Nat. 109:547-563. Medawar, P. B. 1950. Transformation of shape. Proc. Roy. Soc. Land. (B) 137:474-479. Mosimann, J. E. 1970. Size allometry: Size and shape variables with characterizations of the lognormal and generalized gamma distributions. ]. Am. Stat. Assoc. 65:930-945. Mosimann, J. E., and F. C. James. 1979. New statistical methods for allometry with application to Florida red-winged blackbirds. Evolution 33:444-459. Norberg, U. M. 1981. Allometry of bat wings and legs and comparison with bird wings. Philos. Trans. Roy. Soc. Land. (B) 292:359-398. Norberg, U. M., and J. M. V. Rayner. 1987. Ecological morphology and flight in bats (Mammalia; Chiroptera): Wing adaptations, flight performance, foraging strategy and echolocation. Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. Land. (B) 316:335-427. Pagel, M. D., and P. H. Harvey. 1988. The taxon-level problem in the evolution of mammalian brain size: Facts and artifacts. Amer. Nat. 132:344-359. - - . 1989. Taxonomic differences in the scaling of brain on body weight among mammals. Science 244:1589-1593. Pearson, K. 1901. Mathematics and biology. Nature 63:274-275.

STUDY OF ALLOMETRY SINCE HUXLEY

lxxiii

Pedley, T. J.. ed. 1977. Scale Effects in Animal Locomotion. London: Academic Press. Perkkio, J., and R. Keskinen. 1985. The relationship between growth and allometry. ]. Theoret. Biol. 113:81-87. Peters, R. H. 1983. The Ecological Implications of Body Size. New York: Cambridge University Press. Peterson, R. 0., R. E. Page, and K. M. Dodge. 1984. Wolves, moose, and the allometry of population cycles. Science 224:1350-1352. Pezard, A. 1918. Le conditionnement physiologique des caracteres sexuels secondaires chez !es Oiseaux. Bull. Biol. France et Belg. 52: 1-43. - - . 1921. Numerical law of regression of certain secondary sex characters.]. Gen. Physiol. 3:271-297. Phillipson, L. 1981. Bioenergetic options and phylogeny: In Physiological Ecology, ed. C.R. Townsend and P. Calow, 20-45. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications. Prothero, J. 1992. Scaling of bodily proportions in adult terrestrial mammals. Am. ]. Physiol. 262:R492-R503. Quiring, D. P. 1941. The scale of being according to the power formula. Growth 5:301-327. Rainey, R. C., ed. 1976. Insect Flight. New York: John Wiley and Sons. Rayner, J. M. V. 1985. Linear relations in biomechanics: The statistics of scaling functions.]. Zoo!. 206:415-439. Reeve, E. C. R., and J. S. Huxley. 1945. Some problems in the study of allometric growth. In Essays on Growth and Form, Presented to D'Arcy Wentworth Thompson, ed. W. E. Le Gros Clark and P. B. Medawar, 121-156. Oxford: Clarendon Press. Reilly, S. M., and G. V. Lauder, 1990. Metamorphosis of cranial design in tiger salamanders (Ambystoma tigrinum): A morphometric analysis of ontogenetic change.]. Morphol. 204:121-137. Reiss, M. J. 1989. The Allometry of Growth and Reproduction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Rensch, B. 1959. Evolution above the Species Level. New York: Columbia University Press. Reyment, R. A., R. E. Blackith, and N. A. Campbell. 1984. Multivariate Morphometrics. New York: Academic Press. Ricker, W. E. 1973. Linear regressions in fishery research.]. Fish. Res. Bd. Canada 30:409-434. - - . 1984. Computation and uses of central trend lines. Can. ]. Zoo!. 62:1897-1905. Rieppel, 0. 1990. Ontogeny-a way forward for systematics, a way backward for phylogeny. Biol.]. Linn. Soc. 39:177-191. Riska, B. 1991. Regression models in evolutionary allometry. Amer. Nat. 138:283-299. Rohlf, F. J., and F. L. Bookstein. 1987. A comment on shearing as a method of "size correction." Syst. Zoo!. 36:356-367. Schmidt-Nielsen, K. 1984. Scaling: Why Is Animal Size So Important? New York: Cambridge University Press. Shea, B. T. 1985. Bivariate and multivariate growth allometry: Statistical and biological considerations.]. Zoo!. 206:367-390.

lxxiv

PROBLEMS OF RELATIVE GROWTH

Siem, E., and B.-E. Saether. 1983. On rethinking allometry: Which regression model to use?]. Theor. Biol. 104:161-168. Simpson, G. G., A. Roe, and R. C. Lewontin. 1960. Quantitative Zoology. New York: Harcourt Brace and Co. Sleigh, M. A., ed. 197 4. Cilia and Flagella. London: Academic Press. Smith, R. J. 1980. Rethinking allometry. ]. Theoret. Biol. 87:97-111. - - . 198la. Interpretation of correlations in intraspecific and interspecific allometry. Growth 45:291-297. - - . 198lb. On the definition of variables in studies of primate dental allometry. Am. ]. Phys. Anthropol. 55:323-329. Somers, K. M. 1986. Multivariate allometry and removal of size with principal component analysis. Syst. Zool. 35:359-368. Somers, K. M. 1989. Allometry, isometry and shape in principal components analysis. Syst. Zool. 38:169-173. Somerton, D. A. 1980a. Fitting straight lines to Hiatt growth diagrams: A re-evaluation.]. Cons. Int. ExPlor. Mer. 39:15-19. - - . 1980b. A computer technique for estimating the size of sexual maturity in crabs. Can ]. Fish. Aquat. Sci. 37:1488-1494. Sprent, P. 1972. The mathematics of size and shape. Biometrics 28: 23-37. Sprugel, D. G. 1983. Correcting for bias in log-transformed allometric equations. Ecology 64:209-210. Stahl, W. R. 1962. Similarity and dimensional methods in biology. Science 137:205-212. - - . 1967. Scaling of respiratory variables in mammals.]. Appl. Physiol. 22:453-460. Strauss, R. E. 1984. Static allometry and functional feeding morphology in haplochromine cichlids. In Evolution of Fish Species Flocks, ed. A. A. Echelle and I. L. Kornfield, 217-229. Orono: University of Maine Press. - - . 1985. Evolutionary allometry and variation in body form in the South American catfish genus Corydoras (Callichthyidae). Syst. Zool. 34:381-396. - - . 1987. On allometry and relative growth in evolutionary studies. Syst. Zool. 36:72-75. - - . 1992a. Lepidopteran wing morphology: The multivariate analysis of size, shape, and allometric scaling. In Ordinations in the Study of Morphology, Evolution and Systematics of Insects: Applications and Quantitative Genetic Rationales, ed. ]. T. Sorenson and R. G. Foottit, 157-178. Amsterdam: Elsevier Press. - - . 1992b. Developmental variability and heterochronic evolution in poeciliid fishes (Cyprinodontiformes). In Systematics, Historical Ecology, and North American Freshwater Fishes, ed. R. L. Mayden, 492-514. Stanford: Stanford University Press. Strauss, R. E., and R. Altig. 1992. Ontogenetic body form changes in three ecological morphotypes of anuran tadpoles. Growth Devel. Ag. 56:3-16. Strauss, R. E., and F. L. Bookstein. 1982. The truss: Body form reconstruction in morphometrics. Syst. Zool. 31:113-135. Strauss, R. E., and L. A. Fuiman. 1985. Quantitative comparisons of body

STUDY OF ALLOMETRY SINCE HUXLEY

lxxv

form and allometry in larval and adult Pacific sculpins (Teleostei: Cottidae). Can.]. Zool. 63:1582-1589. Sundberg, P. 1989. Shape and size-constrained principal components analysis. Syst. Zool. 38:166-168. Sweet, S. S. 1980. Allometric inference in morphology. Am. Zool. 20:643-652. Teissier, G. 1937. Dysharmonies et Discontinuities dans la Croissance. Paris: Hermann. - - . 1948. La relation d'allometrie: Sa signification statistique et biologique. Biometrics 4:14-53. - - . 1955. Sur la determination de !'axe d'un nuage rectiligne de points. Biometrics 11:344-357. - - . 1960. Relative growth. In The Physiology of the Crustacea, vol. 1, ed. T. H. Waterman, 537-560. New York: Academic Press. Thompson, D. W. 1945 (1917). On Growth and Form. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Thorpe, R. S. 1988. Multiple group principal component analysis and population differentiation.]. Zool. (London) 216:37-40. Turner, M. E. 1978. Allometry and multivariate growth. Growth 42: 434-450. White, J. F., and S. J. Gould. 1965. Interpretation of the coefficient in the allometric equation. Am. Nat. 99:5-18. Wilson, E. 0. 1953. The origin and evolution of polymorphism in ants. Quart. Rev. Biol. 28:136-156. Wingert, F. 1969. Biometrische Analyse der Wachstumsfunktionen von Hirnteilen und Korpergewicht der Albinomaus. ]. Hirnforschung 11:133-197. Wolpoff, M. H. 1982. Relative canine size.]. Human Evol. 10:151-158. Wright, S. 1932. General, group, and special size factors. Genetics 17: 603-619. - - . 1954. The interpretation of multivariate systems. In Statistics and Mathematics in Biology, ed. 0. Kempthorne, T. A. Bancroft, J. W. Gowen, and J. L. Lush, 11-33. Ames: Iowa State College Press. Wu, T. Y. 1977. Introduction to the scaling of aquatic locomotion. In Scale Effects in Animal Locomotion, ed. T. J. Pedley, 203-232. New York: Academic Press. Wu, T. Y.-T., C. J. Brokaw, and C. Brennen, eds. 1975. Swimming and Flying in Nature. New York: Plenum Press. Zar, J. H. 1968. Calculation and miscalculation of the allometric equation as a model in biological data. BioScience 18:1118-1120. Zelditch, M. L. 1987. Evaluating models of developmental integration in the laboratory rat using confirmatory factor analysis. Syst. Zool. 36:368-380. - - . 1988. Ontogenetic variation in patterns of phenotypic integration in the laboratory rat. Evolution 42:28-41. Zelditch, M. L., and A. C. Carmichael. 1989. Ontogenetic variation in patterns of developmental and functional integration in skulls of Sigmodon fulviventer. Evolution 42:814-824.