Semantic dementia - Oxford Journals - Oxford University Press

5 downloads 0 Views 253KB Size Report
and survival in a consecutive series of 100 cases. John R. Hodges,1,2 ... 4 MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, UK ... Cnr Barker & Easy Street,.
doi:10.1093/brain/awp248

Brain 2010: 133; 300–306

| 300

BRAIN A JOURNAL OF NEUROLOGY

Semantic dementia: demography, familial factors and survival in a consecutive series of 100 cases John R. Hodges,1,2 Jo Mitchell,2 Kate Dawson,2 Maria Grazia Spillantini,2 John H. Xuereb,3 Paul McMonagle,3 Peter J. Nestor2 and Karalyn Patterson4 1 2 3 4

Prince of Wales Medical Research Institute (POWMRI), University of New South Wales, Randwick, Sydney, NSW 2031, Australia University Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Addenbrooke’s Hospital, Cambridge, UK Department of Pathology, University of Cambridge, UK MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, UK

Correspondence to: Prof. John R. Hodges, Prince of Wales Medical Research Institute, Cnr Barker & Easy Street, Randwick, NSW 2031, Sydney, Australia E-mail: [email protected]

A great deal has been written about cognitive aspects of semantic dementia but little is known about the demography or prognosis. We describe these features in a consecutive series of 100 patients seen over a 17-year period; all cases were assessed and followed up in a specialist clinic. The mean age at diagnosis was 64.2 (7.1) range 40–79 years, but 46 presented after age 65 and 7 after 75; a higher proportion than the existing literature might predict. Fifteen had a first-degree relative with dementia, but in seven this was almost certainly unrelated. Only two had relatives with young-onset dementia. There were no families with more than two affected members. The familial rate was estimated at between 2% and 7% (95% confidence interval 0–12%). Kaplan–Meier analyses indicated a 50% survival of 12.8 years (95% confidence interval 11.9–13.7); a more benign course than suggested by neuropathologically based studies. We were unable to identify any factors influencing survival. Of the 100, 34 have died, with pathological confirmation in 24; 18 had frontotemporal lobar degeneration with ubiquitin-positive inclusions (13 of 13 confirmed TAR DNA binding protein-43 positive), and 3 had classic tau-positive Pick bodies and 3 had Alzheimer’s pathology. The age at diagnosis or death across the pathological subgroups was equivalent. Although semantic dementia has a strong statistical association with ubiquitin-positive pathology, it does not have the signature of familial frontotemporal lobar degeneration with ubiquitin-positive inclusions, notably the presence of intranuclear lentiform TAR DNA binding protein-43 inclusions. The age of onset is older than predicted and the course more slowly progressive than suggested by earlier studies of small groups of subjects.

Keywords: frontotemporal dementia; semantic dementia; ubiquitin pathology; TDP43 Abbreviations: ACE = Addenbrooke’s cognitive examination; CDR = clinical dementia rating; FTD = frontotemporal dementia; FTLD = frontotemporal lobar degeneration; FTLD-tau = classic tau-positive Pick body frontotemporal dementia; FTLD-U = frontotemporal lobar degeneration with ubiquitin-positive tau-negative inclusions; MMSE = mini-mental state examination; NPI = the neuropsychiatric inventory; TDP = TAR DNA binding protein

Received June 1, 2009. Revised July 30, 2009. Accepted August 22, 2009. Advance Access publication October 5, 2009 ß The Author (2009). Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Guarantors of Brain. All rights reserved. For Permissions, please email: [email protected]

Semantic dementia: a consecutive series of 100 cases

Introduction The syndrome of semantic dementia has been of considerable interest to behavioural neurologists and neuropsychologists since it was first described 100 years ago (Pick, 1892, 1904; Rosenfeld, 1909). In the modern era, interest was re-awakened following Warrington’s (1975) seminal description of three patients with a combination of visual associative agnosia, anomia and disrupted comprehension of word meaning which Warrington conceptualized as secondary to a fundamental breakdown in semantic knowledge (Tulving, 1972). Other cases were subsumed under the label of primary progressive aphasia (Mesulam, 1982, 1987); but the disorder became increasingly recognized as a specific clinical syndrome following the report by Snowden and colleagues (1989) in which they coined the term ‘semantic dementia’. Our paper in 1992 proposed criteria for semantic dementia and made the association between the clinical features and bilateral but asymmetric atrophy of the anterior temporal lobes (Hodges et al., 1992). Over the past 15 years, numerous studies have clarified the clinical neuropsychological features of the syndrome and the implications for understanding cognitive architecture and the neural basis of semantic memory (for review, see Westbury and Bub, 1997; Grossman and Ash, 2004). From a nosological perspective, semantic dementia is now regarded as one of the three main clinical variants of frontotemporal dementia (FTD), sometimes also referred to as frontotemporal lobar degeneration (FTLD) (Neary et al., 1998); the other two are progressive non-fluent aphasia and the behavioural variant of FTD. The neuropathological findings in FTD are varied with a range of tau-positive and tau-negative changes (Hodges et al., 2004; Mackenzie et al., 2006a, b). Recent work has established that the majority of patients with semantic dementia have ubiquitin-positive, tau-negative intraneural inclusions, although occasional cases have classic tau-positive Pick body pathology and a few have Alzheimer’s disease with an atypical distribution of pathology (Davies et al., 2005). In contrast to the extensive cognitive literature on semantic dementia, surprisingly little has been published related to its demography and prognosis. Regarding age of onset, it is commonly held that semantic dementia, in line with other forms of FTD, is a ‘pre-senile’ disorder with onset between the ages of 45 and 65 years. Recent clinicopathological and clinical studies have, however, reported onset after 65 years in 20%–40% of FTD cases (Gislason et al., 2003; Harvey et al., 2003; Ibach et al., 2003, 2004). Reports based on post-mortem series suggested a median survival in semantic dementia of 8 years, range 3–15 (Hodges et al., 2003), but this figure was based on a relatively small sample size (n 4 20) and may have been biased by the restriction to patients reaching autopsy. Another topic of interest is the extent to which semantic dementia is familial. There have been considerable advances in understanding the genetics of FTD with the discovery of two major gene defects (on MAPt and progranulin, both on chromosome 17) plus 2 much rarer loci (CHPM2B and valosin) in familial FTD (Goedert et al., 1998; Skibinski et al., 2005; Baker et al.,

Brain 2010: 133; 300–306

| 301

2006; Cruts et al., 2006; Gasparini et al., 2007; Guinto et al., 2007). Taking FTD as a whole, up to 40% of cases are said to have a positive family history of a neurodegenerative disorder, but in many instances this is probably incidental. Cases with a strong familial tendency (a clear FTD syndrome and/or early-onset dementia in one or more first-degree relatives) are much rarer, perhaps accounting for 10%–20% (Rosso et al., 2003a, b). Moreover, the familial rate appears not to be equally distributed across FTD subtypes. A collaborative cross-centre study from the USA reported a much lower familial rate in semantic dementia compared with other clinical FTD variants (Goldman et al., 2005). This important observation clearly requires further investigation. The aim of the present study was to report the demographic findings on a consecutive series of 100 semantic dementia cases seen over the past 17 years in Cambridge; with an emphasis on the age of presentation, sex distribution and survival. In all patients, we have enquired in detail concerning a potential family history. All patients have been followed up in a specialist clinic until institutionalized, or deceased, allowing us to report on survival and possible presenting factors which might influence prognosis.

Methods The Memory Clinic in Cambridge, UK, was established in 1990 and, from its inception, all cases were classified and recorded on a diagnostic database. In total, between 1990 and 2007, we assessed 3600 new patients in the Memory Clinic, with approximately half of referrals coming from other specialists (neurologists and psychiatrists) and half from general practitioners (GPs) in East Anglia. A final diagnosis of dementia was made in half of these cases, with 416 given a diagnosis of focal cortical dementia: 128 behavioural variant FTD, 110 semantic dementia, 66 progressive non-fluent aphasia, 36 mixed aphasic/behavioural cases and 66 with corticobasal syndrome. All patients with focal cortical syndromes were followed up in a dedicated early onset dementia clinic. As part of the clinic protocol, all patients (with the help of their caregiver) were asked to complete a life and family history questionnaire, developed in Cambridge, prior to their first attendance at the clinic; this enquired about the health and causes of death of all first-degree relatives and, in particular, a history of dementia, Parkinson’s disease, motor neurone disease or major mental health problems. Patients with any positive endorsement of these items were then questioned to obtain further details. All data were entered on the clinic database. Of the 110 with a diagnosis of semantic dementia, 10 were excluded because the data were too limited: the patient either presented at a very advanced stage and therefore had limited follow-up (n = 7) or came from (and returned to) a country other than UK (n = 3). Of the final semantic dementia cohort, 68 were referred by the specialists and 32 by the GPs. We estimated, therefore, that we made a diagnosis of semantic dementia in 4% of the specialist and 1.8% of the GP referrals to our memory clinic. If the rate is calculated only from those diagnosed with dementia, then 8% of the specialist and 3.6% of the GP referrals had semantic dementia. Every effort was made to remain in contact with semantic dementia

302

| Brain 2010: 133; 300–306

J. R. Hodges et al.

patients and their families even when they were no longer able to attend the clinic, and also to enrol cases as potential donors in our Brain Bank Programme, particularly after 1997. Of those with a declaration of intent in vivo, our rate of obtaining brain tissue post-mortem has been 490%. Many of the patients have been included in cognitive imaging or neuropathological reports which have spanned the past 15 years. In all patients identified as semantic dementia, the main presenting pattern was either (i) progressive deterioration of both expressive and receptive vocabulary in the context of relatively fluent and phonologically correct speech production, together with impaired performance on tests of non-verbal semantic knowledge—the classic profile associated with left-predominant temporal lobe atrophy; or (ii) progressive impairment in recognition of people of a crossmodal type affecting identification of faces and names, plus impaired performance on tests of verbal and non-verbal semantic knowledge—a profile which typifies those with right-predominant atrophy (Thompson et al., 2003). All patients showed preservation of day-to-day memory and orientation, working memory, visuospatial and perceptual abilities and non-verbal problem-solving skills (Hodges et al., 1992; Adlam et al., 2006a, b). Structural imaging by MRI revealed focal atrophy in one or both anterior temporal lobes (Galton et al., 2001; Davies et al., 2004; Galton et al., 2001; Williams et al., 2005). In total, 94 patients had MRI scanning (the other six had CT only, for a variety of reasons). In all 94, sufficient information was available on the distribution of the anterior temporal atrophy to allow classification as left- or right-predominant atrophy. All patients underwent a detailed neuropsychological evaluation with tests designed to assess the following domains: semantic memory (category fluency, picture naming, word–picture matching and picture–picture associative tasks using the Cambridge Semantic Battery) (Bozeat et al., 2000; Hodges and Patterson, 2007), verbal and visual episodic memory, working memory, problem solving, visuospatial and visuoperceptual abilities. The exact tests changed somewhat over the 17 year period of case acquisition and the results will not be reported here: most of these findings have been published elsewhere. All patients had the mini-mental state examination (MMSE) and, after 1997, the Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination (ACE) (Mathuranath et al., 2000; Mioshi et al., 2006). A carer-based schedule, the Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR), was used to assess the severity of impairment in everyday skills. The Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI: Cummings, 1997), introduced in 1997, was completed for 73 patients.

Results Demographic and clinical data Of the 100 cases, 60 were men and 40 women. They were a relatively well-educated group with a mean of 12.3  2.9 years full-time education: 38 had a professional background, 28 were skilled workers, 16 semi-skilled and 14 manual workers (we failed to obtain occupational information for n = 3). The mean age at onset of symptoms, as related by family members, was 60.3 (7.1) years with a range of 40–79 and the mean age at diagnosis was 64.2 (7.1), indicating an average lag of 4 years between symptom onset and diagnosis (Table 1). As shown in Fig. 1, the modal age for diagnosis was between 60 and 64 years. Only one patient was aged under 50. Of note was the fact that in 46 patients, the diagnosis was made after the age of 65 years, in 7 after 75 years and in 1 after 80 years. The mean MMSE at presentation was 21.0 (4.4) and the CDR score 0.76 (0.5). There was a modest endorsement on the NPI

Figure 1 Distribution of age at diagnosis for all 100 semantic dementia cases.

Table 1 Comparison of basic demographic data of the 94 cases with a known pattern of atrophy on MRI Characteristic

Whole group

Left

Right

M/F Years of education MMSE CDR NPI (n = 73) Age at onset Age at diagnosis Age into NH (n = 30) Age at death (n = 32) 50% survival (95% CI) from onset Median survival

60/40 12.3  2.9 21.0  6.4 0.8  0.5 20.4  16.5 60.3  7.01 64.24  7.1 66.9  6.5 69.7  5.8 12.7 (11.9–13.7) 14.0 (12.5–15.5)

38/32 12.4  3.1 20.2  6.67 0.7  0.5 18.2  15.5 60.3  7.1 64.5  7.2 67.0  6.4 69.5  5.6 13.0 (11.9–14,1) 14.0 (12.8–15.2)

18/6 11.6  2.4 24.0  4.6 0.7  0.5 26.4  18.0 59.3  6.5 63.1  6.6 66.6  7.1 70.1  6.6 11.7 (10.3–13.4) 12.0 (9.8–14.2)

CI = confidence interval.

Semantic dementia: a consecutive series of 100 cases

Brain 2010: 133; 300–306

| 303

typically involving apathy and repetitive behaviours with a mean score of 20.4 (16.5). Table 1 compares basic demographic data of the 94 cases with a known pattern of atrophy on MRI. Of these 94 cases, 70 had left- and 24 right-predominant atrophy. The sex distributions, years of education, ages at onset and at diagnosis were almost identical in the two subgroups. The MMSE was higher in the right-predominant group (24.0  4.6 versus 20.2  6.7) but the difference did not reach significance. The right group had higher scores on the NPI (26.4  18.0 versus 18.2  15.5) indicating a greater incidence of neuropsychiatric symptoms, but this difference also did not reach significance.

Family history A family history was taken in all 100 cases as described earlier: 15 had a family history of dementia in a first-degree relative. In 7 of the 15 cases, there was sufficient evidence to make an alternative diagnosis outside the FTD spectrum including vascular dementia, alcoholism and longstanding Parkinson’s disease. In the remaining 8, a diagnosis of Alzheimer’s or senile dementia had been made, but information on these family members was relatively scanty as most had died many years before. In one patient, we were able to obtain post-mortem confirmation that his mother had Alzheimer’s disease with an onset in her seventies, leaving seven possible familial cases. Only two patients (2%) had a clear family history of early onset dementia or ‘Pick’s disease’ but without pathological confirmation in either instance. There were no families with more than two affected members. The rate of a suggestive family history was, therefore, between 2% and 7% (95% confidence interval (CI) 0–12%).

Survival analyses and pathology To date, 32 of the 100 patients have died at a mean age of 69.7  5.8 years. Pathological confirmation was available in 24 of the 32: 18 had FTLD with ubiquitin-positive tau-negative inclusions (FTLD-U), 3 had classic tau-positive Pick body FTD (FTLD-tau) and 3 Alzheimer pathology: 18 of the 24 cases were included in a prior study by Davies et al. (2005). The pattern of ubiquitin-positive staining corresponded to type 2 in the Mackenzie et al. (2006) classification, with ubiquitin-positive neurites in layer II of the cortex and in the dentate gyrus but without intranuclear ‘cat’s eye’ inclusions. Of the 18 with ubiquitin-positive inclusions, we were able to retrieve sufficient material to re-stain for TAR DNA binding protein (TDP)-43 in 13 cases. In brief, 8 mm paraffin sections were rehydrated and boiled in 10 mM trisodium citrate–HCl buffer pH 6 for antigen retrieval. Sections were incubated overnight at 4 C with primary antibody polyclonal antisera anti-TDP43 (1-2000; ProteinTech Inc.). Staining was developed using the Vectastain biotin–avidin ABC Elite Kit (Vector Laboratories) and visualized using 3,3’-diaminobenzidine (DAB) as substrate (Vector Labs). Sections were counterstained with cresyl violet, all of which were positive. Usually round or curved cytoplasmic TDP43 inclusions were present in the dentate fascia of the hippocampus.

Figure 2 TDP43 staining in a case of semantic dementia. Anti-TDP43 antibody stains numerous cytoplasmic inclusions in the dentate gyrus (A) and some cytoplasmic inclusions as well as neuritis in frontal cortex (B). Normal nuclear TDP43 staining can also be observed. Scale Bar in B for A and B = 55 mm.

Similar inclusions and TDP43 dystrophic neurites were present in frontal and mainly in temporal cortices. Representative illustrations of TDP43 staining of the dentate gyrus and frontal cortex are shown in Fig. 2. The age at diagnosis and death was equivalent across the three pathological subgroups: FTLD-U 63.5 and 71.5 years; FTLD-tau 65.3 and 69.6 years; Alzheimer’s disease 67.2 and 71.5 years, respectively. Of note is the fact that our oldest patient, who presented at the age of 77 years and died at the age of 88 years, had FTLD-U as did four others diagnosed in their seventies. One patient with a presentation in their 70s had FTLD-tau. The three patients with Alzheimer’s disease pathology were aged 60, 63 and 79 at the time of diagnosis and died aged 64, 66 and 85. Review of clinical, neuropsychological and imaging data revealed no consistent differences between the pathological subgroups. Of the surviving 68, 34 are in institutional care and 34 are living at home and under follow-up in the clinic.

304

| Brain 2010: 133; 300–306

Figure 3 Cumulative survival curve of all 100 semantic dementia (SD) patients. Disease duration is given in years and ‘x’ denotes censored cases

Kaplan–Meier survival analysis of the whole group (Fig. 3) indicated a median survival of 12.8 years (95% CI 11.9–13.7). Many patients have spent a considerable number of years in nursing homes. We performed a number of analyses in an attempt to identify clinical predictors of survival. There was no difference in survival between those with left- or right-predominant atrophy. Somewhat surprisingly, severity of dementia at presentation as judged by either score on the MMSE, CDR, naming performance on the semantic battery or total NPI score did not significantly affect the survival. There were also no effects of gender, age at presentation, education or occupation.

Discussion The data reported on this series of 100 semantic dementia cases, seen in a single centre over 17 years, represent the most extensive demographic survey undertaken in this syndrome. Both the mean age at symptom onset (60.3  7.1) and at diagnosis (64.2  7.1) was older than predicted and it is notable that in 46 patients the diagnosis was made after the age of 65 years, and in 7 after the age of 75 years. These findings suggest that semantic dementia may be a more common cause of dementia in later life than is commonly thought and, if seen in non-specialist centres, such patients might well be diagnosed with Alzheimer’s disease. All patients were followed up in a dedicated clinic until the time of institutionalization or death which allowed us to obtain an accurate estimate of survival. Kaplan–Meier-based analysis indicated a 50% survival rate of 12.8 years. Rather surprisingly, none of the factors that we were able to assess, including severity of dementia or of anomia at presentation, had a significant impact on survival.

J. R. Hodges et al. A total of 32 patients have died and we obtained post-mortem confirmation of the pathological diagnosis in 24: 18 had FTLD with ubiquitin-positive tau-negative inclusions (FTLD-U), 3 had classic tau-positive Pick body FTLD and 3 Alzheimer pathology. These findings extend a prior report on 18 semantic dementia cases (Davies et al., 2005). The genetic influence on the aetiology of semantic dementia appears to be low. In our cohort, 15% reported a first-degree relative with dementia, but in seven cases this was almost certainly unrelated and one had pathologically confirmed Alzheimer’s disease. Only two had relatives with early onset dementia and there were no multiplex families. Bearing in mind the late onset of many of our cases, it is possible that a proportion of the elderly relatives diagnosed as Alzheimer’s disease might have had semantic dementia. Even with this proviso, the maximum rate of positive family history was 7% (95% CI 0–12%). As discussed earlier, FTD is generally regarded as a pre-senile dementia syndrome. Research has identified FTD as the second commonest neurodegenerative cause of dementia in the under 65s (Ratnavalli et al., 2002) with a prevalence in that age range which approaches that of Alzheimer’s disease. This study suggests that it might, in fact, be a significant cause of dementia in those diagnosed beyond 65. Of note is the fact that in six patients presenting over the age of 70 years, we have pathological confirmation of an FTLD syndrome (5 FTLD-U and 1 FTLD-tau). In contrast of the three with pathological Alzheimer’s disease, two were aged under 65 and one was 79-years old. There seems no way, therefore, of predicting pathology on the basis of age. Estimates based on specialist clinics are, of course, open to bias. The 100 semantic dementia patients reported here represent a quarter of all those with FTD syndromes which, in turn, was the diagnosis in 20% of 1800 patients with dementia seen over a 17 year period. Since the primary complaint of most semantic dementia patients is ‘loss of memory’ and they perform poorly on tests of verbal episodic memory—which forms the mainstay of dementia screening tests—it is easy to imagine why patients could be diagnosed with Alzheimer’s disease unless fuller neuropsychological testing and/or magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) are performed. Clues to the diagnosis, on the positive side, are well-preserved orientation, excellent visuospatial skills and good non-verbal episodic memory; and, with regard to notable impairments, poor category fluency, naming and word comprehension plus surface dyslexia. This profile has been shown to produce a characteristic pattern of impairment and preservation on the ACE, which is a 15 min cognitive screening tool readily applicable for use in a general memory clinic (Mioshi et al., 2006). Coronal MRI consistently shows anterior temporal lobe atrophy involving the polar, parahippocampal and inferior temporal regions with striking involvement of the anterior fusiform gyrus, a tell-tale sign universally present in semantic dementia (Galton et al., 2001; Davies et al., 2004) that may potentially have diagnostic utility in excluding apparent clinical semantic dementia patients who have Alzheimer pathology (Pereria et al., 2009). The average delay between symptom onset and diagnosis was 4 years and did not differ between those with predominant rightversus left-temporal atrophy. The preponderance of patients with

Semantic dementia: a consecutive series of 100 cases left more than right-temporal atrophy was noted by earlier authors (Hodges, 2007), but the cause of this asymmetry has remained mysterious. The findings here that left more than right and right more than left subgroups had similar ages of onset and similar delays between onset and diagnosis, fail to support one previous hypothesis that the deficits of face and voice recognition seen in right-predominant cases are less noticeable to family members than the striking anomia, which characterizes left-predominant semantic dementia (Thompson et al., 2003). The cause for this striking biological asymmetry awaits explanation. There have been substantial advances in our understanding of the neuropathology and genetics of FTD. Although it is claimed that up to 40% of patients with FTD are genetically determined, a previous multicentre study established very different rates across different syndromes in the FTD spectrum, with a low rate for semantic dementia (Goldman et al., 2005). A large survey from the Netherlands also found a much lower rate of an autosomal dominant inheritance pattern in patients with semantic dementia (4%) compared with those with behavioural variant FTD subtypes (36%) (Seelaar et al., 2008). We have confirmed here that relatively few semantic dementia patients appear to have a significant family history. All patients completed a life and family history questionnaire and in 15 there was a history of dementia in a firstdegree relative. In eight of these, we were able to establish an alternative aetiology leaving seven with a possible family history. Only two had a history of younger-onset dementia but given our finding of a surprisingly large number of semantic dementia patients with an onset over 65 (many of whom have pathologically confirmed FTLD-U or FTLD-tau), it is possible that some of those with late-onset dementia had a semantic dementia or a related form of FTD. The overall estimate of familiarity was between 0% and 12%. There were no cases with a family history suggestive of an autosomal dominant pattern of inheritance typically defined as three affected individuals in two generations (Seelaar et al., 2008). It is now known that a proportion of patients with familiar FTD associated with ubiquitin-positive inclusions have mutations of the progranulin gene on chromosome 17 (Baker et al., 2006; Cruts et al., 2006). But interestingly, there have been no reports of semantic dementia in association with progranulin mutations. Moreover, the pattern of ubiquitin staining with intranuclear ‘cat’s eye’ inclusions which characterizes familial FTLD-U is not found in semantic dementia (Mackenzie et al., 2006a). It appears, therefore, that semantic dementia is largely a sporadic disorder, and that families can be re-assured of a low risk of genetic transmission. The survival in semantic dementia is highly variable. We have seen patients who died with widespread dementia within 2 or 3 years of diagnosis, while others have lived for a decade or more. Our current longest survivor is 15 years post-diagnosis. We had hoped to establish prognostic variables able to predict slow versus fast decline. Despite comprehensive analysis of the dataset, no significant predictors emerged. Most surprisingly, severities of dementia as judged by score on the MMSE or ACE, degree of anomia or neuropsychiatric disturbance do not appear to be predictive. Further work to determine predictors of survival and to identify the pathological substrate in vivo are required.

Brain 2010: 133; 300–306

| 305

Funding Medical Research Council Program grant (to J.R.H); Australian Research Council Federation Fellowship (FF0776229 to J.R.H.); British Neuropathological Society Grant (to M.G.S. and M.S.).

References Adlam A.-L.R., Bozeat S, Arnold R, Watson P, Hodges JR. Semantic knowledge in mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer’s disease. Cortex 2006a; 42: 675–84. Adlam A-LR, Patterson K, Rogers TT, Nestor PJ, Salmond CH, Acosta-Cabronero J, et al. Semantic dementia and fluent primary progressive aphasia: Two sides of the same coin? Brain 2006b; 129: 3066–80. Baker M, Mackenzie IR, Pickering-Brown SM, Gass J, Rademakers R, Lindholm C, et al. Mutations in progranulin cause tau-negative frontotemporal dementia linked to chromosome 17. Nature 2006; 442: 916–9. Bozeat S, Lambon Ralph MA, Patterson K, Garrard P, Hodges JR. Nonverbal semantic impairment in semantic dementia. Neuropsychologia 2000; 38: 1207–15. Cruts M, Gijselinck I, van der Zee J, Engelborghs S, Wils H, Pirici D, et al. Null mutations in progranulin cause ubiquitin-positive frontotemporal dementia linked to chromosome 17q21. Nature 2006; 442: 920–4. Cummings JL. The Neuropsychiatric Inventory: assessing psychopathology in dementia patients. Neurology 1997; 48: S10–S16. Davies R, Graham KS, Xuereb JH, Williams GB, Hodges JR. The human perirhinal cortex and semantic memory. Eur J Neurosci 2004; 20: 2441–6. Davies RR, Hodges JR, Kril J, Patterson K, Halliday G, Xuereb J. The pathological basis of semantic dementia. Brain 2005; 128: 1984–95. Galton CJ, Patterson K, Graham K, Lambon-Ralph MA, Williams G, Antoun N, et al. Differing patterns of temporal atrophy in Alzheimer’s disease and semantic dementia. Neurology 2001; 57: 216–25. Gasparini L, Terni B, Spillantini MG. Frontotemporal dementia with tau pathology. Neurodegener Dis 2007; 4: 236–53. Gislason TB, Sjogren M, Larsson L, Skoog I. The prevalence of frontal variant frontotemporal dementia and the frontal lobe syndrome in a population based sample of 85 year olds. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatr 2003; 74: 867–71. Goedert M, Crowther RA, Spillantini MG. Tau mutations cause frontotemporal dementias. Neuron 1998; 21: 955–8. Goldman JS, Farmer JM, Wood EM, Johnson JK, Boxer A, Neuhaus J, et al. Comparison of family histories in FTLD subtypes and related tauopathies. Neurology 2005; 65: 1817–9. Grossman M, Ash S. Primary progressive aphasia: A review. Neurocase 2004; 10: 3–18. Guinto JB, Ritson GP, Taylor JP, Forman MS. Valosin-containing protein and the pathogenesis of frontotemporal dementia associated with inclusion body myopathy. Acta Neuropathol 2007; 114: 55–61. Harvey RJ, Skelton-Robinson M, Rossor MN. The prevalence and causes of dementia in people under the age of 65 years. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2003; 74: 1206–9. Hodges JR. Frontotemporal dementia syndromes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2007. Hodges JR, Davies R, Xuereb J, Kril J, Halliday G. Survival in frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 2003; 61: 349–54. Hodges JR, Davies RR, Xuereb JH, Casey B, Broe M, Bak TH, et al. Clinicopathological correlates in frontotemporal dementia. Ann Neurol 2004; 56: 399–406. Hodges JR, Patterson K. Semantic dementia: a unique clinicopathological syndrome. Lancet Neurol 2007; 6: 1004–14.

306

| Brain 2010: 133; 300–306

Hodges JR, Patterson K, Oxbury S, Funnell E. Semantic dementia. Progressive fluent aphasia with temporal lobe atrophy. Brain 1992; 115 (Pt 6): 1783–806. Ibach B, Koch H, Koller M, Wolfersdorf M. Hospital admission circumstances and prevalence of frontotemporal lobar degeneration: a multicenter psychiatric state hospital study in Germany. Demen Geriatr Cogn Disord 2003; 16: 253–64. Ibach B, Poljansky S, Barta W, Koller M, Wittmann M, Hajak G. Patterns of referring of patients with frontotemporal lobar degeneration to psychiatric in- and out-patient services. Results from a prospective multicentre study. Demen Geriatr Cogn Disord 2004; 17: 269–73. Mackenzie IR, Baborie A, Pickering-Brown S, Du Plessie D, Jaros E, Perry RH, et al. Heterogeneity of ubiquitin pathology in frontotemporal lobar degeneration: classification and relation to clinical phenotype. Acta Neuropathol 2006a; 112: 539–49. Mackenzie IR, Baker M, West G, Woulfe J, Qadi N, Gass J, et al. A family with tau-negative frontotemporal dementia and neuronal intranuclear inclusions linked to chromosome 17. Brain 2006b; 129 (Pt 4): 853–67. Mathuranath PS, Nestor P, Berrios GE, Rakowicz W, Hodges JR. A brief cognitive test battery to differentiate Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 2000; 55: 1613–20. Mesulam MM. Primary progressive aphasia—differentiation from Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Neurol 1987; 22: 533–4. Mesulam MM. Slowly progressive aphasia without generalized dementia. Ann Neurol 1982; 11: 592–8. Mioshi E, Dawson K, Mitchell J, Arnold R, Hodges JR. The Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination Revised (ACE-R): a brief cognitive test battery for dementia screening. Int J Geriartr Psychiatr 2006; 21: 1078–85. Neary D, Snowden JS, Gustafson L, Passant U, Stuss D, Black S, et al. Frontotemporal lobar degeneration: a consensus on clinical diagnostic criteria. Neurology 1998; 51: 1546–54. Pereria JM, Williams GB, Acosta-Cabaronero J, Pengas G, Spillantini MG, Xuereb JH, et al. Atrophy patterns in histologic vs clinical groupings of frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Neurology 2009; 72: 1653–60.

J. R. Hodges et al. Pick A. Uber die Beziehungen der senilen Hirnatrophie zur Aphasie. Prager Medische Wochenschrift 1892; 17: 165–7. Pick A. Zur symptomatologie der linksseitigen Schlafenlappenatrophie. Monatschrift fur Psychiatrie und Neurologie 1904; 16: 378–88. Ratnavalli E, Brayne C, Dawson K, Hodges JR. The prevalence of frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 2002; 58: 1615–21. Rosenfeld M. Die partielle Gorsshirnatrophie. Journal fur Psychologie und Neurologie 1909; 14: 115–30. Rosso SM, Donker Kaat L, Baks T, Joosse M, de Koning I, Pijnenburg YA, et al. Frontotemporal dementia in The Netherlands: patients characteristics and prevalence estimates from a population-based study. Brain 2003; 126: 2016–122. Rosso SM, Landweer EJ, Houterman M, Donker Kaat L, van Duijn CM, van Swieten JC. Medical and environmental risk factors for sporadic frontotemporal dementia: a retrospective case-control study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2003; 74: 1574–6. Seelaar H, Kamphorst W, Azmani A, Masdjedi R, de Koning I, MaatKievit JA, Anar B, et al. Distinct genetic forms of frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 2008; 71: 1220–6. Skibinski G, Parkinson NJ, Brown JM, Chakrabarti L, Lloyd SL, Hummerich H, et al. Mutations in the endosomal ESCRTIII-complex subunit CHMP2B in frontotemporal dementia. Nat Genet 2005; 37: 806–8. Snowden JS, Goulding PJ, Neary D. Semantic dementia: a form of circumscribed cerebral atrophy. Behav Neurol 1989; 2: 167–82. Thompson SA, Patterson K, Hodges JR. Left/right asymmetry of atrophy in semantic dementia: behavioral-cognitive implications. Neurology 2003; 61: 1196–203. Tulving E. Episodic and semantic memory. In: Tulving E, Donaldson W, editors. Organisation of memory. New York: Academic Press; 1972. p. 381–403. Warrington EK. The selective impairment of semantic memory. Q J Exp Psychol 1975; 27: 635–57. Westbury C, Bub D. Primary progressive aphasia: a review of 112 cases. Brain Lang 1997; 60: 381–406. Williams GB, Nestor PJ, Hodges JR. The neural correlates of semantic and behavioural deficits in frontotemporal dementia. Neuroimage 2005; 24: 1042–51.