What do symptoms mean? | The BMJ

2 downloads 67794 Views 53KB Size Report
Aug 21, 2003 - ... are agreeing to our use of cookies. Find out more here Close. Access provided by Google Indexer. Subscribe. My Account. My email alerts.
Editorials Indigenous health services Indigenous health services provide a range of healing methods, including conventional professional services and traditional healing. Probably their most significant contribution is improved access to health services for indigenous people, enabling earlier intervention, energetic outreach, higher levels of compliance, and a greater sense of community participation and ownership. Indigenous services tend to be built around indigenous philosophies, aspirations, social networks, and economic realities. As noted in the New Zealand health strategy, development of the workforce is a common theme in the development of indigenous health. Maori make up around 14% of the total population but only 5% of the national health workforce.7 On equity grounds, indigenous participation in the professional health workforce should match community demographic profiles and may challenge health educators to review admission criteria and the content of curriculums. Indigenous health workers are often employed as cultural or community aids bringing first hand knowledge of the community and a capacity to engage reluctant patients. However, they should also be well enough versed in health issues to make informed decisions about patients’ referral and management. Otherwise there is potential for professional and cultural interventions to diverge. While there is some debate about which approach is likely to produce the best results, in practice conventional services and indigenous services can exist comfortably together. More pertinent is the type of service that is going to be most beneficial to meet a particular need. In general, indigenous health services are more convincing at the level of primary health care. Higher rates of childhood immunisation, for example, sem to be possible with services that are closely linked to indigenous networks, and early intervention is embraced with greater enthusiasm when offered by indigenous providers.8

Conventional health services and indigenous services need, however, to work together within a collaborative framework.9 Clinical acumen will be sharpened by cultural knowledge, and community endeavours will be strengthened by access to professional expertise. It makes sense to build health networks that encourage synergies between agencies, even when philosophies differ. Because indigenous services are well placed to foster greater awareness of health issues, sponsor a stronger sense of self management, and promote healthy lifestyles, they represent innovative and important components of the health care system, complementing other initiatives. Competing interests: None declared.

Mason Durie professor of Maori research and development Massey University, Private Bag 11-222, Palmerston North, New Zealand ([email protected])

1 2 3

4

5

6 7

8

9

Kunitz S. Disease and social diversity the European impact on the health of nonEuropeans. New York: Oxford University Press, 1994. King A. The New Zealand health strategy. Wellington: Ministry of Health, 2000. National Advisory Committee on Health and Disability. The social, cultural and economic determinants of health in New Zealand: action to improve health. Wellington: National Health Committee, 1998. Ajwani S, Blakely T, Robson B, Tobias M, Bonne M. Decades of disparity— ethnic mortality trends in New Zealand 1980–1999. Wellington: Ministry of Health, 2003. Durie MH, Gillies A, Kingi Te K, Ratima MM, Waldon J, Morrison PS, et al. Guidelines for purchasing personal mental health services for Maori. Palmerston North: Department of Maori Studies, Massey University, 1995. Castillo RJ. Culture and mental illness: a client centred approach. Los Angeles: Brooks/Coles, 1997. Health Workforce Advisory Committee. The New Zealand Health workforce—a stocktake of issues and capacity. Wellington: Health Workforce Advisory Committee; 2001. Waldrum JB, Herring DA, Young TK. Aboriginal health in Canada— historical, cultural and epidemioloical perspectives. Toronto: University of Toronto Press; 1997. Gray D, Saggers S, Hulse G, Atkinson D. An approach to substance misuse problems among indigenous Australians. In: Hulse G, White J, Cape G, eds. Management of alcohol and drug problems. Melbourne: Oxford University Press, 2002:310-27.

What do symptoms mean? Symptoms should be explained in the broader perspective of the patient’s cognition and mood Papers pp 423 and 426

BMJ 2003;327:409–10

BMJ VOLUME 327

D

octors often turn to psychology when trying to understand patients and their problems, especially when no underlying physical pathology can be found. They focus on depression and anxiety as causes of symptoms and propose therapies such as counselling and cognitive behaviour therapy as possible solutions. The symptom itself is rarely questioned. But is the symptom so unproblematic? Why does one person experience headaches and sore throats whereas another has migraines and tonsillitis? Why does retirement exacerbate symptoms but a busy job make them disappear? And why do some patients bring their headaches to the doctor whereas others

23 AUGUST 2003

bmj.com

manage their migraines at home? An understanding of how symptoms arise and how only some are given the status of a problem can create a broader psychological perspective in which to understand patients’ health and illness. In this issue Mercer et al and Nazareth et al describe “lacking interest in sex” as the problem reported most often.1 2 In their anxiety not to feed into the medicalisation of sexual dysfunction they emphasise the role of psychological factors in exacerbating lowered libido and describe possible psychological solutions. This approach draws on the tradition of clinical psychology, with its focus on the identification 409

Editorials management of psychological problems. Lack of sexual interest, for example, may relate to depression and anxiety, and such affective states may be implicated in both the cause and the treatment of the problem. But this analysis accepts the symptom as presented. The symptom is considered simply an indicator of an underlying (psychological) problem, which is divorced from the idiosyncratic patient. This approach cannot explain why symptoms vary so much between individuals and even in the same person as her or his lifestyle and social situation changes. The alternative is to regard the symptom not as a simple sensation but as a perception that places the patient’s experience within a broader perspective that includes not only mood but also cognition. Research on the perception of symptoms highlights what some of these cognitions might be.3-5 Before patients can perceive that they have a symptom such as “lacking interest in sex” the salience of their experience needs to be raised above a threshold of recognition. Many elderly people (and people of all ages in fact) would not routinely monitor their level of sexual interest and do not think, “I am aware of my level of sexual interest.” If sexual interest is of no relevance, then, however low, it will not be detected. Once detected, the symptom then needs to be labelled as normal or a problem—“my level of sexual interest should be higher.” For a patient to detect and label a symptom a range of cognitions are required. Many have been described and manipulated by much psychological research. For example, cognitions include attention, value, social and peer norms, self identity, and an analysis of cost and benefit. These are reflected in thoughts such as “I am aware of my libido,” “The media tell me that every one is having sex therefore having a libido is important,” “Everyone in the media seems to be having sex all the time so I must have a problem,” “My friends are having more sex than me,” “I am a sexual being,” “There is no point wanting sex as I can’t get it,” and “I would rather sleep than have sex.” Central to all these cognitions is patients’ appraisal of the symptom in the context of their own and others’ life experience. This appraisal may, firstly, raise the salience of the experience to create a symptom, and, secondly, result in the symptom being labelled a problem. Even a questionnaire or interview about sexual interest could raise the salience

410

of the problem. However, the role of cognitions does not stop here. When a symptom has both been experienced and been labelled a problem, appraisal also influences whether or not a patient seeks help. To visit the doctor for “lacking interest in sex” the patient has to believe that the problem is of relevance to medicine, that a doctor is the appropriate person to see, that he or she would be prepared to do what the doctor suggests, and finally that he or she actually wants the problem removed. Some patients may both perceive the symptom and decide that it is a problem but also decide that this problem works for them. Symptoms are brought to doctors in their consulting rooms and described by patients in research studies. Doctors have a duty to rule out an organic cause that can be treated by a medical solution. When this has been achieved they sometimes turn to narrowly defined psychology to broaden their understanding of the patient. But the symptom itself remains unexplored. The very generation of a symptom and the translation of the patient’s perception into a problem are also open to psychological influence. An understanding of the range of cognitions that create, exacerbate, and label symptoms could provide doctors with a richer forum for understanding the variability in the way patients present and the complexities of their experience. Despite the findings of the two papers, there is unlikely to be any need to get alarmed about the high levels of lack of sexual interest reported. Symptoms are not that simple. Jane Ogden reader in health psychology Department of General Practice and Primary Care, Guy’s King’s and St Thomas’ School of Medicine, London SE11 6SP ([email protected])

Competing interests: None declared. 1

2

3

4

5

Mercer CH, Fenton KA, Johnson AM, Wellings K, Macdowall W, McManus S, et al. Sexual function problems and help seeking behaviour in Britain: national probability sample survey. BMJ 2003:327:426-7. Nazareth I, Boynton P, King M. Problems with sexual function in people attending London general practitioners: cross sectional study. BMJ 2003:327:423-6. Pennebaker JW. Accuracy of symptom perception. In: Baum A, Taylor SE, Singer J, eds. Handbook of psychology and health. Vol 4. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1983. Van Zuuren FJ. The effects of information, distraction and coping style on symptom reporting during preterm labour. Psychol Health 1998:13: 49-54. Skelton JA, Pennebaker JW. The psychology of physical symptoms and sensations. In: Sanders GS, Suls J, eds. Social psychology of health and illness. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1982.

BMJ VOLUME 327

23 AUGUST 2003

bmj.com